全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2052篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2091篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 20篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 23篇 |
2020年 | 54篇 |
2019年 | 57篇 |
2018年 | 61篇 |
2017年 | 69篇 |
2016年 | 64篇 |
2015年 | 58篇 |
2014年 | 66篇 |
2013年 | 223篇 |
2012年 | 104篇 |
2011年 | 114篇 |
2010年 | 81篇 |
2009年 | 75篇 |
2008年 | 82篇 |
2007年 | 99篇 |
2006年 | 89篇 |
2005年 | 76篇 |
2004年 | 69篇 |
2003年 | 75篇 |
2002年 | 75篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 31篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有2091条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
The discussions which follow rest on a distinction, first expounded by Husserl, between formal logic and formal ontology. The former concerns itself with (formal) meaning-structures; the latter with formal structures amongst objects and their parts. The paper attempts to show how, when formal ontological considerations are brought into play, contemporary extensionalist theories of part and whole, and above all the mereology of Leniewski, can be generalised to embrace not only relations between concrete objects and object-pieces, but also relations between what we shall call dependent parts or moments. A two-dimensional formal language is canvassed for the resultant ontological theory, a language which owes more to the tradition of Euler, Boole and Venn than to the quantifier-centred languages which have predominated amongst analytic philosophers since the time of Frege and Russell. Analytic philosophical arguments against moments, and against the entire project of a formal ontology, are considered and rejected. The paper concludes with a brief account of some applications of the theory presented. 相似文献
102.
Lisa R. Fournier Jonathan M. Gallimore Kevin R. Feiszli Gordon D. Logan 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2014,21(1):163-169
When we plan sequences of actions, we must hold some elements of the sequence in working memory (WM) while we execute others. Research shows that execution of an action can be delayed if it partly overlaps (vs. does not overlap) with another action plan maintained in WM (partial repetition cost). However, it is not known whether all features of the action maintained in WM interfere equally with current actions. Most serial models of memory and action assume that interference will be equal, because all action features in the sequence should be activated to an equal degree in parallel; others assume that action features earlier in the sequence will interfere more than those later in the sequence, because earlier features will be more active. Using a partial repetition paradigm, this study examined whether serial position of action features in action sequences maintained in WM have an influence on current actions. Two stimulus events occurred in a sequence, and participants planned and maintained an action sequence to the first event (action A) in WM while executing a speeded response to the second event (action B). Results showed delayed execution of action B when it matched the first feature in the action A sequence (partial repetition cost), but not when it matched the last feature. These findings suggest that serial order is represented in the action plan prior to response execution, consistent with models that assume that serial order is represented by a primacy gradient of parallel feature activation prior to action execution. 相似文献
103.
Odell S Sander E Denson LA Baldassano RN Hommel KA 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2011,18(1):39-45
The objective of this study was to examine the relative contributions of both parental and adolescent functioning to family functioning in adolescent patients with inflammatory bowel disease (IBD) and their families. Participants were 45 adolescents (27 male, 18 female) 13-17?years old (M?=?15.41?years, SD?=?1.32) with IBD and their parents. Families completed measures of patient behavioral functioning and depression, parent distress and family functioning. Disease severity assessments were completed via data provided by patients' gastroenterologists. Results indicated that parent-reported patient behavioral problems accounted for a significant 26% of variance in family functioning. Post-hoc analysis revealed that externalizing behavior problems accounted for the majority of this variance compared to internalizing behavior problems. These results suggest that externalizing problems may have a more significant impact on these families than previous research indicates. Moreover, externalizing behaviors may significantly impact family adaptation and should be taken into consideration during routine clinical care. Further research is needed to replicate and expand upon these findings. 相似文献
104.
The influence of athletic experience and kinematic information on skill-relevant affordance perception 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Weast JA Shockley K Riley MA 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(4):689-706
Humans can perceive affordances both for themselves and for others, and affordance perception is a function of perceptual-motor experience involved in playing a sport. Two experiments investigated the enhanced affordance perception of athletes. In Experiment 1, basketball players and nonbasketball players provided perceptual reports for sports-relevant (maximum standing-reach and reach-with-jump heights) and non-sports-relevant (maximum sitting height) affordances for self and other. Basketball players were more accurate at perceiving maximum reach-with-jump for another person than were nonbasketball players, but were no better at perceiving maximum reach or sitting heights. Experiment 2 investigated the informational basis for this enhanced perceptual ability of basketball players by evaluating whether kinematics inform perceivers about action-scaled (e.g., force-production dependent), but not body-scaled (i.e., geometrically determined), affordances for others, and whether basketball experience enhances sensitivity to kinematic information. Only basketball players improved at perceiving an action-scaled affordance (maximum reach-with-jump), but not body-scaled affordances (maximum standing-reach and sit) with exposure to kinematic information, suggesting that action-scaled affordances may be specified by kinematic information to which athletes are already attuned by virtue of their sport experience. 相似文献
105.
Jessie E. Menzel Steffanie L. Sperry Brent Small J. Kevin Thompson David B. Sarwer Thomas F. Cash 《Sex roles》2011,65(7-8):469-477
The Tripartite Influence Model of Body Image was adapted to examine the role of body satisfaction, perceived pressure to have cosmetic surgery, and internalization of societal appearance ideals in understanding cosmetic surgery attitudes. Participants were 2,048 men (N?=?445) and women (N?=?1,603) American college students from Florida who completed a range of measures that assessed levels of body satisfaction, perceived appearance pressures, internalization of appearance standards, and cosmetic surgery attitudes. A structural equation model was used to test hypothesized relations independently for men and women. Results indicated a moderate-good fit to the data, with both internalization and body satisfaction mediating the effect of perceived pressures on cosmetic surgery attitudes. Invariance testing revealed significant differences in pathway estimates between samples of men and women. The findings offer further support for the Tripartite Influence Model of Body Image and indicate potential factors that may influence cosmetic surgery attitudes. 相似文献
106.
Thomas?A.?Fergus Kevin?D.?WuEmail author 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2011,33(4):446-458
Using two large nonclinical samples (N = 725), relations between five targeted cognitive variables [intolerance of uncertainty, negative problem orientation, perfectionism/certainty,
responsibility/threat estimation, and importance/control of thoughts] and mood [depression] and anxiety [social anxiety, generalized
anxiety, and obsessive–compulsive] symptoms were examined. Analyses provided multiple levels of specificity, including zero-order
correlations, partial correlations controlling for the effects of positive and negative affect, regression analyses, and hierarchical
structural modeling. Results were that (a) intolerance of uncertainty showed relative specificity to anxiety versus depression
symptoms and (b) negative problem orientation was common to mood and anxiety symptoms. Although certain analyses suggested
that (c) perfectionism/certainty specifically predicted generalized anxiety and (d) both responsibility/threat estimation
and importance/control of thoughts were unique predictors of obsessive–compulsive symptoms, these three cognitive variables
inconsistently predicted symptom scores across the two studies. Conceptual and therapeutic implications are discussed. 相似文献
107.
Kevin Anderson 《Studies in East European Thought》1992,44(2):79-129
I would like to thank the following people for helpful comments and criticisms: Janet Afary, Paul Buhle, Bud Burkhard, Olga Domanski, Nigel Gibson, Peter Hudis, Martin Jay, Douglas Kellner, Andrew Kliman, Pierre Lantz, Heinz Osterle, Jim Thomas, Lou Turner, and Alan Wald. 相似文献
108.
Resource-limited regions of the world represent the areas most affected by the global HIV epidemic. Currently, there are insufficient
data on the neurocognitive effects of HIV in these areas and neuropsychological studies that have been carried out thus far
are marked by inconsistent methods, test batteries, and rating systems for levels of cognitive impairment. These differences
in methods, along with genetic variability of both virus and host, differences in co-infections and other co-morbidities,
differences in language and culture, and infrastructural deficiencies in many international settings create challenges to
the assessment of neurocognitive functioning and interpretation of neuropsychological data. Identifying neurocognitive impairment
directly attributable to HIV, exploring relationships between HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment, disease variables,
and everyday functioning, evaluating differences in HIV-1 subtype associated neuropathology, and determining implications
for treatment remain complicated and challenging goals. Endeavors to establish a more standardized approach to neurocognitive
assessments across international studies in addition to accumulating appropriate normative data that will allow more accurate
rating of neuropsychological test performance will be crucial to future efforts attempting to achieve these goals. 相似文献
109.
Kevin Hart 《Sophia》2009,48(4):435-459
This essay seeks to explore contemplation as it features in Christian theology and philosophy, both ancient and modern. Contemplation,
in ancient philosophy, is transformed in Christian theology; nonetheless, it has the structure of what Jean Wahl calls ‘transascendance’,
a rising to the heights. Although contemplation remains as a theme in modern Christian theology, it drops out in modern philosophy:
that is, post-Renaissance philosophy. And yet it returns, both in analytic and continental philosophy, in the twentieth century.
It returns, however, in the mode of ‘transdescendance’: by way of conditions of possibility, and fundamental orientations. 相似文献
110.
Based on the standard error of measurement, Holland (1985) suggested the “rule of eight” for determining the meaningfulness of differences between two summary scores on the Self Directed Search. The present study empirically examined the rule's validity for practice. The participants were 2397 (1497 females and 900 males) undergraduate students enrolled in the exploratory major at a large Midwestern university. The results strongly supported the interpretive rule of eight. For those participants who scored a primary-code distinction of eight or more points, the hit rate was 55% with a kappa of .38. This compared to a hit rate of 40% with a kappa of .21 for those participants with less than eight-point distinction between their top two scores. The authors also discuss the practical implications for interpreting SDS results using the “rule of eight.” 相似文献