全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2363篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2396篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 24篇 |
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 60篇 |
2019年 | 70篇 |
2018年 | 69篇 |
2017年 | 75篇 |
2016年 | 74篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 80篇 |
2013年 | 247篇 |
2012年 | 116篇 |
2011年 | 135篇 |
2010年 | 86篇 |
2009年 | 80篇 |
2008年 | 93篇 |
2007年 | 106篇 |
2006年 | 104篇 |
2005年 | 86篇 |
2004年 | 76篇 |
2003年 | 83篇 |
2002年 | 86篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2396条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
902.
Research on children with ADHD indicates an association with inaccuracy of self-appraisal. This study examines the accuracy of self-evaluations in clinic-referred adults diagnosed with ADHD. Self-assessments and performance measures of driving in naturalistic settings and on a virtual-reality driving simulator are used to assess accuracy of self-evaluations. The group diagnosed with ADHD (n= 44) has a higher rate of collisions, speeding tickets, and total driving citations in their driving history; report less use of safe driving behaviors in naturalistic settings; and use fewer safe driving behaviors in the simulator than the community comparison group (n= 44). Despite poorer performance, adults with ADHD provide similar driving self-assessments, thereby overestimating in naturalistic settings to a greater degree than the comparison group. These findings extend research in children with ADHD to an adult sample in an important domain of functioning and may relate to findings of executive deficits associated with ADHD. 相似文献
903.
Kathleen Galek Neal Krause Christopher G. Ellison Taryn Kudler Kevin J. Flannelly 《Journal of Adult Development》2007,14(1-2):16-25
The current study examined the connections among religious doubt, mental health, and aging in a nationwide sample of individuals
18 years and older (N = 1629). Findings indicate that that religious doubt emerging from the recognition of suffering and evil in the world has
a deleterious impact on mental health. Results also show that as people grow older, religious doubts continue to be associated
with psychopathology, but the magnitude of this association becomes weaker across age categories. In other words, the impact
of doubt on mental distress declines as one ages. These effects were found across various measures of mental distress, including
depression, general anxiety, interpersonal sensitivity, paranoia, hostility, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Findings are
explored within the context of psychosocial development and the experiential trajectory of religious maturity. 相似文献
904.
Common and distinct factors in decision making under ambiguity and risk: A psychometric study of individual differences 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Marco Lauriola Irwin P. Levin Stephanie S. Hart 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2007,104(2):130-149
The relation between decision making under ambiguity and risky decision making was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, choices under ambiguity were measured for a large sample receiving an Ellsberg-type Ambiguity-Probability Tradeoff Task. Participants with extreme scores were recruited for Part 2 of each study which consisted of a risky decision making task (Study 1) or a series of decisions under ambiguity in “real life” scenarios (Study 2). Despite a time gap of up to 2 months, individual differences in scores on Part 1 predicted scores on Part 2. In Study 3 participants received in a single session several risky decision making tasks, several measures of decision making under ambiguity, and several personality scales related to uncertainty and decision making style. Taken together, the findings support the existence of a stable dispositional trait to reduce uncertainty in decision making but also task-specific differences related to gains and losses. 相似文献
905.
Sue DW Capodilupo CM Torino GC Bucceri JM Holder AM Nadal KL Esquilin M 《The American psychologist》2007,62(4):271-286
Racial microaggressions are brief and commonplace daily verbal, behavioral, or environmental indignities, whether intentional or unintentional, that communicate hostile, derogatory, or negative racial slights and insults toward people of color. Perpetrators of microaggressions are often unaware that they engage in such communications when they interact with racial/ethnic minorities. A taxonomy of racial microaggressions in everyday life was created through a review of the social psychological literature on aversive racism, from formulations regarding the manifestation and impact of everyday racism, and from reading numerous personal narratives of counselors (both White and those of color) on their racial/cultural awakening. Microaggressions seem to appear in three forms: microassault, microinsult, and microinvalidation. Almost all interracial encounters are prone to microaggressions; this article uses the White counselor--client of color counseling dyad to illustrate how they impair the development of a therapeutic alliance. Suggestions regarding education and training and research in the helping professions are discussed. 相似文献
906.
In a longitudinal study using the Washington University Sentence Completion Test (WUSCT), we examined changes in ego development in response to the events of September 11, 2001. A sample of 24 individuals responded to the WUSCT in both the summer and fall of 2001, before and after the events of September 11. A significant decrease in mean ego level was found following September 11, as well as more equivocal support for an increase in variability in responses to the WUSCT. 相似文献
907.
We investigated a test of strategic reasoning (the Windows task) that in different studies has yielded contrasting pictures of young children's executive abilities [Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The “windows task” as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349; Samuels, M. C., Brooks, P. J., & Frye, D. (1996). Strategic game playing through the windows task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 159–172]. An experiment with 52 three- to four-year-olds showed robust effects of different wordings for the prompts used to ask children to respond, and found that a single exposure to the facilitating wording led to improved performance on subsequent trials where the standard wording was used. This suggests that the effect of the wording was to help children infer an appropriate basis for responding, and not to reduce the trial-by-trial working memory or inhibitory demands of the task. 相似文献
908.
Sharon L Manne Jamie Ostroff Gary Winkel Generosa Grana Kevin Fox 《Health psychology》2005,24(6):635-641
This 18-month longitudinal study examined the associations among partner unsupportive behavior, avoidant coping, and distress experienced by 219 women with early stage breast cancer. The role of patient and partner ratings of unsupportive behavior were evaluated. Results indicated that patient and partner ratings of unsupportive behavior were highly correlated. Growth curve modeling suggested that unsupportive behavior, from both patient and partner perspectives, predicted more avoidant coping and distress. When partner and patient perceptions were placed in the same model, patient perceptions mediated the association between partners' ratings of their unsupportive behavior and patient distress. Avoidance also mediated the association between unsupportive behavior and distress, extending prior cross-sectional findings. Results highlight the long-term detrimental effects of partners' unsupportive behavior on the quality of life of women with early stage breast cancer. 相似文献
909.
910.
Kevin E. O’grady 《Behavior research methods》1982,14(3):359-360