首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   545篇
  免费   24篇
  国内免费   1篇
  570篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   17篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   42篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   22篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   20篇
  2006年   21篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   9篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   8篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   16篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   9篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   6篇
  1972年   5篇
  1970年   4篇
  1968年   5篇
排序方式: 共有570条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Previous studies of natural palatability-sensitive reactions, elicited from rats by tastes, have indicated that the taste of concentrated NaCl becomes more palatable during states of body sodium depletion. A training procedure based upon “sensory preconditioning” or “irrelevant incentive” designs was used here to establish gustatory conditioned labels (quinine or citric acid) for either NaCl or fructose while rats were in a normal physiological state. Experiment 1 replicated demonstrations by others that gustatory conditioned labels for salt can attract and act as independent incentives during the salt appetite induced by sodium depletion. Experiment 2 used the taste reactivity measure to show that the enhanced palatability of the taste of NaCl transfers to produce enhanced palatability of the taste of the isolated conditioned label for salt in a state-dependent fashion. These results offer further support for the proposition that conditioned incentives not only predict hedonic events to follow, but themselves become attributed with the hedonic properties of their reinforcers.  相似文献   
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.

Purpose

The purpose of this study was to investigate the conditional effects of power values diversity and relationship conflict.

Design/Methodology/Approach

We utilized a time-lagged survey design and multilevel modeling to investigate 60 teams working on a project task over the course of 4 months.

Findings

When participative safety climate was high, the presence of high power values diversity was particularly helpful for reducing relationship conflict. In turn, decreased relationship conflict tended to increase team performance. Additionally, when workload sharing was low, high relationship conflict was especially harmful to team performance.

Implications

Results support the consideration of team participative safety climate to better understand the conditions under which power values diversity is likely to lessen relationship conflict and subsequently increase team performance. Findings also highlight the importance of avoiding low workload sharing, in the presence of prominent relationship conflict, to increase team performance.

Originality/Value

By examining relationship conflict as a mediator and participative safety climate as a moderator of power values diversity’s effects, we make a novel contribution to extant literature by helping to elucidate both how and under what conditions differences in power values, among team members, can influence team performance. Relatedly, we answer the call for more research that adopts a contingency approach toward examining the effects of values diversity and relationship conflict. In doing so, we help to identify the conditions under which power values diversity and relationship conflict are likely to differentially influence important team outcomes.
  相似文献   
47.
Trait anxiety is an individual-difference variable reflecting variation in state-anxiety elevations resulting from exposure to a stressor. It is usually measured using questionnaire instruments, such as the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T). The present research conceptually distinguishes, and independently assesses, two hypothetical dimensions of anxiety vulnerability which, it is argued, could plausibly make independent contributions to variance in trait-anxiety scores. These dimensions are anxiety reactivity, the probability of experiencing an anxiety reaction to a stressor, and anxiety perseveration, the persistence of anxiety symptoms once elicited. Participants were asked three questions about each STAI-T item. The traditional STAI-T question assessed how much of the time this symptom was experienced; the anxiety-reactivity question assessed the probability of experiencing the symptom in response to a stressor; and the anxiety-perseveration question assessed how long the symptom persisted, if elicited. Regression analysis determined that anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration scores both accounted for independent variance in trait-anxiety scores. It is argued that models of anxiety vulnerability should seek to differentiate both the causes and the consequences of elevated anxiety reactivity and increased anxiety perseveration. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).  相似文献   
48.
49.
Two common strategies for successful foraging are learning to associate specific sensory cues with patches of prey ("associative learning") and using set decision-making rules to systematically scan for prey ("algorithmic search"). We investigated whether an animal's life history affects which of these two foraging strategies it is likely to use. Natterer's bats (Myotis nattereri) have slow life-history traits and we predicted they would be more likely to use associative learning. Common shrews (Sorex araneus) have fast life-history traits and we predicted that they would rely more heavily on routine-based search. Apart from their marked differences in life-history traits, these two mammals are similar in body size, brain weight, habitat, and diet. We assessed foraging strategy, associative learning ability, and retention time with a four-arm maze; one arm contained a food reward and was marked with four sensory stimuli. Bats and shrews differed significantly in their foraging strategies. Most bats learned to associate the sensory stimuli with the reward and remembered this association over time. Most shrews searched the maze using consistent decision-making rules, but did not learn or remember the association. We discuss these results in terms of life-history traits and other key differences between these species. Our results suggest a link between an animal's life-history strategy and its use of associative learning.  相似文献   
50.
Genuine moral disagreement exists and is widespread. To understand such disagreement, we must examine the basic kinds of social relationships people construct across cultures and the distinct moral obligations and prohibitions these relationships entail. We extend relational models theory (Fiske, 1991) to identify 4 fundamental and distinct moral motives. Unity is the motive to care for and support the integrity of in-groups by avoiding or eliminating threats of contamination and providing aid and protection based on need or empathic compassion. Hierarchy is the motive to respect rank in social groups where superiors are entitled to deference and respect but must also lead, guide, direct, and protect subordinates. Equality is the motive for balanced, in-kind reciprocity, equal treatment, equal say, and equal opportunity. Proportionality is the motive for rewards and punishments to be proportionate to merit, benefits to be calibrated to contributions, and judgments to be based on a utilitarian calculus of costs and benefits. The 4 moral motives are universal, but cultures, ideologies, and individuals differ in where they activate these motives and how they implement them. Unlike existing theories (Haidt, 2007; Hauser, 2006; Turiel, 1983), relationship regulation theory predicts that any action, including violence, unequal treatment, and "impure" acts, may be perceived as morally correct depending on the moral motive employed and how the relevant social relationship is construed. This approach facilitates clearer understanding of moral perspectives we disagree with and provides a template for how to influence moral motives and practices in the world.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号