全文获取类型
收费全文 | 55715篇 |
免费 | 1043篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
56766篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 565篇 |
2019年 | 663篇 |
2018年 | 863篇 |
2017年 | 885篇 |
2016年 | 1011篇 |
2015年 | 711篇 |
2014年 | 838篇 |
2013年 | 4120篇 |
2012年 | 1570篇 |
2011年 | 1602篇 |
2010年 | 982篇 |
2009年 | 1043篇 |
2008年 | 1402篇 |
2007年 | 1353篇 |
2006年 | 1210篇 |
2005年 | 1090篇 |
2004年 | 1041篇 |
2003年 | 997篇 |
2002年 | 1039篇 |
2001年 | 1622篇 |
2000年 | 1613篇 |
1999年 | 1171篇 |
1998年 | 551篇 |
1997年 | 516篇 |
1992年 | 1031篇 |
1991年 | 997篇 |
1990年 | 958篇 |
1989年 | 944篇 |
1988年 | 900篇 |
1987年 | 851篇 |
1986年 | 881篇 |
1985年 | 964篇 |
1984年 | 800篇 |
1983年 | 740篇 |
1982年 | 551篇 |
1981年 | 571篇 |
1979年 | 872篇 |
1978年 | 627篇 |
1977年 | 571篇 |
1976年 | 573篇 |
1975年 | 738篇 |
1974年 | 855篇 |
1973年 | 903篇 |
1972年 | 762篇 |
1971年 | 714篇 |
1970年 | 659篇 |
1969年 | 698篇 |
1968年 | 858篇 |
1967年 | 782篇 |
1966年 | 748篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
E H Adelson J Jonides 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1980,6(3):486-493
Some simple models of iconic storage, based on the persisting responses of photoreceptors, were tested in two experiments. Substantial changes in such physical parameters as adapting luminance, stimulus luminance, and stimulus contrast produced little or no change in the duration of partial-report performance. This is at odds with most receptoral models. It seems unlikely that any model based on receptoral persistence can accommodate the results, thus forcing one to locate the icon beyond the receptors and probably beyond the retina as a whole. 相似文献
872.
Degree of ear asymmetries for perception of dichotic chords and for illusory chord localization in musicians of different levels of competence 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Left-ear superiority for perception of dichotically presented musical chords was seen in musicians at all levels of competence. However, a hypothesis that dominance would be greater in the more professional musicians was not confirmed. Whereas many of the professionals did have the largest left-ear preferences, about half had strong right-ear superiorities instead. On the average, therefore, there was no ear dominance for the two most professional groups, although the other groups had the expected left-ear dominance. Furthermore, ear differences for the professional groups were distributed only in the categories of weak to strong left- and weak to strong right-ear preferences, whereas ear differences for the other groups included also the central categories of no or little ear preference. The better musician groups were also more strongly affected by an auditory illusion in which right-ear dominance occurred when subjects reported the ear receiving the high chord, whereas left-ear dominance was seen when they reported the ear receiving the low chord (for the same dichotic pair). That is, ear dominance was dependent on instructions, switching in the same subject from left to right ear when the task changed from reporting the location of the low to reporting the location of the high chord, respectively. However, ear dominance and switching were only partially present in nonmusicians and were weak in amateurs. It may be that ear dominance for chord recognition is influenced by specialized cognitive development in musicians, on the one hand, and by noncognitive neurophysiological factors on the other. 相似文献
873.
874.
875.
Prime-time television programs were analyzed for frequencies of positive reinforcement and punishment exchanged among performers varying in age and sex. Results indicated that most responses were given by and directed to adult males. Moreover, while adult males exhibited and received similar frequencies of reinforcement and punishment, adult females were likely to give and to be the recipients of more reinforcement than punishment. Proportional analyses which controlled for sex differences in frequencies of responses revealed that females more often exhibited and received reinforcement, whereas males more often exhibited and received punishment. Results are interpreted from a social learning theory perspective and the implications for children's learning of positive and negative behaviors are explored. 相似文献
876.
877.
878.
H. Tristram Engelhardt Jr. 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1980,1(1):39-50
The ways in which ethical issues arise in making clinical judgments are briefly discussed. By showing the topography of the role of value judgments in medical diagnostics it is suggested why clinical medicine remains inextricably a value-infected science. 相似文献
879.
Gordon L. Mangan Timothy J. H. Paisey 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1980,15(4):159-171
This paper reviews the theory and research of the Warsaw school of differential psychophysiology, which has modified and extended the typologic model constructed by Teplov and Nebylitsyn. While Soviet theory still favors a relatively inflexible structure of nervous system properties as the biologic basis of individual differences in personality, Strelau and colleagues, in line with the action/activity model of Vygotsky and Tomaszewski, begin with the premise that for every individual there is a specific, genetically determined optimal level of arousal, a necessary background for fullest emotional and intellectual development. The person strives to create this optimal climate through an active process of stimulation control, achieved primarily through enhancement or dampening of reactivity—which describes innate responsivity in the reacting systems to both sensory and emotional stimuli—through various hormonal ‘tuning’ mechanisms and through “appropriate” activity, reflected in different cognitive styles and preference for certain work conditions. Such activities aim to ensure comfortable physical and psychologic environments, in which the individual can avoid conflict and stress. The system regulating and integrating such control mechanisms is the core of personality. Investigations of the Warsaw group on relationships between reactivity and different forms of stimulation seeking/control—risk-taking, work style preferences, cognitive style, defense mechanisms, for example—and between reactivity and tolerance of stress and conflict and their speculations about the neurophysiologic and endocrinologic mechanisms of stimulation control are described, as are a number of questionnaires developed by this group to measure reactivity, the temporal parameters of response, and stimulation-seeking in a variety of occupational and social contexts. 相似文献
880.
A. H. Reinhardt-Rutland 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,28(6):569-571
It is suggested that aftereffects caused by swept change of sound level (Reinhardt-Rutland & Anstis, Note 1) may contribute to auditory motion aftereffects (Grantham & Wightman, 1979). The latter appear to show selectivity for frequency; they are substantial if the frequency is .5 kHz but not if it is 2 kHz. An experiment was carried out to show to what extent there is such selectivity for aftereffects from swept sound-level change; this showed that they are substantial over a much wider range of frequencies than auditory motion aftereffects. It is concluded that the frequency selectivity of auditory motion aftereffects might be explained by frequency selectivity for inter-aural phase differences. 相似文献