全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2260篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2288篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 32篇 |
2019年 | 42篇 |
2018年 | 36篇 |
2017年 | 51篇 |
2016年 | 40篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 41篇 |
2013年 | 221篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 90篇 |
2010年 | 42篇 |
2009年 | 51篇 |
2008年 | 104篇 |
2007年 | 76篇 |
2006年 | 87篇 |
2005年 | 67篇 |
2004年 | 73篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 86篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 40篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 29篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 31篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 29篇 |
1981年 | 33篇 |
1980年 | 23篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 33篇 |
1977年 | 40篇 |
1976年 | 28篇 |
1975年 | 32篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1970年 | 18篇 |
1966年 | 20篇 |
排序方式: 共有2288条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Rautakoski P Hannus T Simberg S Sandnabba NK Santtila P 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》2012,37(3):202-210
The present study explored the prevalence of self-reported stuttering in a Finnish twin population and examined the extent to which the variance in liability to stuttering was attributable to genetic and environmental effects. We analyzed data of 1728 Finnish twins, born between 1961 and 1989. The participants were asked to complete a questionnaire on speech, language, and voice. In two of the questions they were asked to report the occurrence of childhood and present stuttering of their own and that of their sibling. According to the results, 2.3% (52) of the participants were reported to have stuttered as children and 28.8% of them (15) were reported to continue to stutter in adulthood. There was no significant gender difference in the prevalence of stuttering in either childhood or adulthood. For childhood stuttering, the tetrachoric correlation was higher for monozygotic pairs (r=.74) than for dizygotic pairs (r=.27). By means of structural equation modeling it was found that 82% of the variance in liability to childhood stuttering was attributable to additive genetic effects, with the remaining 18% due to non-shared environmental effects. In conclusion, the results of the present study confirm findings from prior studies and support a strong genetic and only a moderate non-shared environmental effect on stuttering. Potential small differences in the prevalence of stuttering in different populations are suggested by our data. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to recognize the contribution of genetic and environmental effects on stuttering. 相似文献
82.
Undermining the Zeigarnik effect: Another hidden cost of reward 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Forty college students participated in a study ostensibly designed to obtain measures of the hemispheric activity while they worked on a spatial reasoning task. In fact, our true interest was in determining whether subjects would return to the spatial reasoning task once the hemispheric recordings were completed. Subjects would normally be expected to return to the task by virtue of the Zeigarnik effect because no subject completed the task during the “hemispheric recording” phase. The manipulation involved telling one group of subjects that they would be paid $1.50 for participating in the study (expected reward group). The remainder of the subjects were not led to expect the reward (unexpected reward group). The result was that 86% of the unexpected reward subjects but only 58% of the expected reward subjects (p < .05) manifested the Zeigarnik effect. This differential tendency to return to the task was further reflected in time differences. The average unexpected reward subject spent 3 min 48 sec of a five-minute free-choice period at the task, whereas the average expected reward subject spent only 2 min 20 sec (p < .05). Reward expectancy, therefore, led to an undermining of the Zeigarnik effect. This observation supports Condry's prediction that rewarding performance at a task can lead to premature task disengagement. 相似文献
83.
Some extensions of the Bradley-Terry-Luce model for paired comparisons are presented and used to assess the existence of a home court advantage for professional basketball teams. It is shown how maximum likelihood estimates for these models can be obtained from a simple iterative scaling algorithm. Won-loss records for the regular season are used to make predictions for playoff series. The models are extended to include possible differences among divisions. These models can be applied in many other situations to incorporate group effects and order effects into analysis of paired comparisons. 相似文献
84.
In mirror-reading, words are read from right to left and letters are read in a reverse right-to-left orientation. In one experiment we compared the ability of normal right- and left-handed subjects to mirror-read and found that the left-handers made fewer errors and could read mirror print more rapidly. In a second experiment we attempted to learn whether there is a hemifield superiority for reading mirror words and whether there are any differences between left- and right-handers in a hemifield. We found that although both right- and left-handers more rapidly detected mirror words projected to the left visual half-field, there were no differences between groups. However, in the right visual half-field, the performance of left-handers was superior to that of the right-handers. The results of the hemifield study suggest that left-handers may be superior at reading mirror words because they can more easily reverse their scanning pattern. 相似文献
85.
86.
Kenneth V. Hardy PhD 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1993,15(1):9-20
This article asserts that the family therapy field is approaching an epistemological shift from structuralism and positivism to postmodernism and relativism. The confluence of these movements: feminism, constructivism/constructionism, and cultural relativism is cited as a major impetus for the shift. Live supervision is examined within the context of this transformation, with a discussion of implications for the future. 相似文献
87.
88.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
89.
Kenneth T. Wang Austin M. Johnson Yanmei Cao Li Zhang Allison Parsley 《Journal of Beliefs & Values》2019,40(1):104-121
The Attitudes Toward Religion Scale (ATRS) was developed to access the level of interest in and position toward the five state-approved religions in China. Data were analysed across two samples of Chinese college students. With Sample 1 (N = 278), exploratory factor analyses were used to select the 10 ATRS items corresponding to two factors: Interest and Position. With Sample 2, confirmatory factor analyses (N = 270) cross-validated the two-factor oblique model as well as a bifactor model. Cronbach alphas of ATRS subscale scores in the two samples ranged from .75 to .85. ATRS-Interest and Position were both positively associated with number of close relationships with religious people. In addition, negative attitudes toward religion was associated with being religiously proselytised by strangers. Moreover, female students reported more favourable attitudes toward religion compared to their male counterparts. The overall results support ATRS as a psychometrically strong and promising measure. 相似文献
90.
Kenneth A. Chase Dominique Treboux K. Daniel O'Leary Zvi Strassberg 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1998,26(6):467-473
Ninety-five high-risk adolescents were studied to determine whether their dating aggression and its justification as a response to interpersonal problems were specific to the current partner, general to dating relationships, or part of a global age-mate (same-sex peers and opposite-sex dating partners) aggression problem. Approximately one-third of males and two-thirds of females reported physical aggression against their current dating partner. Males' aggression (and its justification) toward their current dating partner was part of a generalized pattern of dating aggression, whereas for females, physical aggression against a current dating partner (and its justification) was partner-specific and unrelated to aggression in other relationships. Findings are discussed with regard to intervention and future research on adolescent dating aggression. 相似文献