全文获取类型
收费全文 | 56402篇 |
免费 | 960篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
57367篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 767篇 |
2019年 | 977篇 |
2018年 | 1084篇 |
2017年 | 1100篇 |
2016年 | 1337篇 |
2015年 | 968篇 |
2014年 | 968篇 |
2013年 | 4725篇 |
2012年 | 1717篇 |
2011年 | 1889篇 |
2010年 | 1094篇 |
2009年 | 1107篇 |
2008年 | 1722篇 |
2007年 | 1669篇 |
2006年 | 1488篇 |
2005年 | 1344篇 |
2004年 | 1323篇 |
2003年 | 1197篇 |
2002年 | 1247篇 |
2001年 | 1774篇 |
2000年 | 1683篇 |
1999年 | 1296篇 |
1998年 | 705篇 |
1997年 | 602篇 |
1996年 | 546篇 |
1995年 | 544篇 |
1994年 | 530篇 |
1993年 | 546篇 |
1992年 | 1037篇 |
1991年 | 979篇 |
1990年 | 956篇 |
1989年 | 879篇 |
1988年 | 834篇 |
1987年 | 827篇 |
1986年 | 824篇 |
1985年 | 884篇 |
1984年 | 687篇 |
1983年 | 611篇 |
1982年 | 497篇 |
1979年 | 671篇 |
1978年 | 521篇 |
1976年 | 473篇 |
1975年 | 620篇 |
1974年 | 648篇 |
1973年 | 614篇 |
1972年 | 550篇 |
1971年 | 488篇 |
1968年 | 558篇 |
1967年 | 560篇 |
1966年 | 495篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
Rats were given backward pairings of a tone (CS) and shock (US) and were then tested for response to the CS in a lick suppression test. Animals given a cuing or reminder treatment prior to the test exhibited enhanced suppression in the presence of the tone relative to a variety of control conditions. The cue-induced suppression enhancement did not appear to result from sensitization or short-term motivational changes nor did it seem to depend on second-order conditioning of the test context. The effect appeared more robust as the number of backward conditioning trials preceding the cue increased. The results were discussed in terms of current explanations of backward conditioning effects. 相似文献
232.
White KG Pipe ME McLean AP Redman S 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1985,44(2):207-215
Distributions of reinforcers between two components of multiple variable-interval schedules were varied over a number of conditions. Sensitivity to reinforcement, measured by the exponent of the power function relating ratios of responses in the two components to ratios of reinforcers obtained in the components, did not differ between conditions with 15-s or 60-s component durations. The failure to demonstrate the “short-component effect,” where sensitivity is high for short components, was consistent with reanalysis of previous data. With 60-s components, sensitivity to reinforcement decreased systematically with time since component alternation, and was higher in the first 15-s subinterval of the 60-s component than for the component whose total duration was 15 s. Varying component duration and sampling behavior at different times since component transition may not be equivalent ways of examining the effects of average temporal distance between components. 相似文献
233.
Jean M. Blomquist M.A. 《Pastoral Psychology》1985,34(2):82-91
This essay presents the findings from a study of the effect of divorce on spiritual growth among Protestants. Looking at movement in the areas of love of God, self, and others, it shows that the divorce experience can have varying levels of impact which relate at least partially to the traumatic intensity of the experience itself.Excerpted from The Effect of Divorce on Spiritual Growth: A Study in Protestant Spirituality (unpublished thesis), (c) 1984 Jean M. Blomquist. All rights reserved. 相似文献
234.
M J Howes J E Hokanson D A Loewenstein 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1985,49(4):1110-1113
Scores on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) were periodically obtained from the roommates of college students who exhibited a persistent mild depression over a 3-month period. For comparative purposes, BDI scores were also obtained from roommates of individuals who were transiently depressed and from subjects with nondepressed roommates. In comparison with control subjects, the roommates of persistently depressed persons displayed a progressive increase in BDI score over the course of the study. 相似文献
235.
Two perspectives on the nature of the social group and psychological group formation are discussed. The traditional social cohesion approach traces group formation to processes of interpersonal attraction, while the social identity approach defines the group in cognitive terms and considers identification, or self-categorization, to be the mechanism of psychological group formation. On the basis of an experiment by Turner, Sachdev and Hogg (1983) it is hypothesized that interpersonal attraction (positive or negative) is related to group formation only in so far as it enhances intergroup distinctiveness. This hypothesis is experimentally tested in a 2 × 3 (interpersonal liking/disliking per se versus no explicit categorization/random categorization/criterial categorization on the basis of affect) factorial design employing the ‘minimal group’ paradigm. People who like each other and were not explicitly categorized formed a group. This effect was enhanced by criterial categorization but disappeared when categorization was random. Although the results do not support the hypothesis, they are not explicable in social cohesion terms. A social identity explanation is furnished—attraction influences group formation by acting, under certain specifiable conditions, as a cognitive criterion for common category membership. This explanation is located in current theorizing and is proposed as part of a reconceptualization of the relationship between interpersonal attraction and group formation. 相似文献
236.
Additional results from Rutte and Wilke's (1984) study are presented. In the context of a social dilemma situation two variables were manipulated: use and variance. Preference for various decision structures is the issue of the present paper. It appeared that a large majority structure and an unanimity structure were mast preferred. A small majority structure and a structure in which each decides for him/herself were intermediately preferred. Whereas a structure in which one person, a leader, decides for all was least preferred. Further analysis of these data suggested that only the latter two decision structures were influenced by the use and variance manipulations. 相似文献
237.
238.
Eric J. Oritt Stephen C. Paul Jay A. Behrman 《American journal of community psychology》1985,13(5):565-582
The development and psychometric investigation of the Perceived Support Network Inventory (PSNI), a measure of perceived social support, is described. A group of 146 introductory psychology students participated in a test-retest study design that collected reliability, internal consistency, and construct, convergent, and discriminant validity data on the scale. In addition, a contrast group of 28 Counseling Center clients was administered the PSNI. Test-retest reliability of the PSNI total score and subscale scores ranged from .72 to .88. Internal consistency for the PSNI was .77. Construct validity estimates ranged from .21 to .57. Convergent validity estimates varied from -.25 to .20. Discriminant validity estimates varied from -.11 to .19. Sample differences between introductory psychology student mean PSNI total and subscale scores and Counseling Center client mean PSNI total and subscale scores proved significant. Recommendations for subsequent research and future application are offered. 相似文献
239.
Richard M. Ryckman Michael A. Robbins Bill Thornton Joel A. Gold Robert H. Kuehnel 《Journal of research in personality》1985,19(3):288-298
In his theorizing about self-actualization, Maslow speculated that physical ability and physical self-confidence were two aspects of cooperative dominance which were associated positively with actualization. These aspects of dominance bear close resemblance to the Perceived Physical Ability (PPA) and the Physical Self-Presentation Confidence (PSPC) dimensions of the Physical Self-Efficacy (PSE) Inventory. Accordingly, the PPA and PSPC scores of undergraduate men and women were correlated with their scores on the Personal Orientation Inventory (POI), a popular measure of actualization, to test Maslow's hypotheses. The results showed that, contrary to Maslow's claims, PPA is not a positive correlate of self-actualization. However, strong support was found for Maslow's contention that high-PSPC individuals are more likely to be actualizing than low-PSPC people. Also, Maslow's hypothesis that actualizers are high in global sensation seeking (SS) was tested. The data indicated that only male actualizers were high in various aspects of SS, but female actualizers were not. Multiple regression analyses revealed further that both SS and PSPC are primary contributors to POI for men, but only PSPC contributes significantly to POI for women. Discussion focused on the reasons why research findings showing that increases in physical fitness lead to increases in self-esteem cannot be used to support the view that physical fitness and actualization are linked positively. 相似文献
240.