全文获取类型
收费全文 | 57513篇 |
免费 | 973篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
58491篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 798篇 |
2019年 | 996篇 |
2018年 | 1117篇 |
2017年 | 1134篇 |
2016年 | 1372篇 |
2015年 | 984篇 |
2014年 | 1001篇 |
2013年 | 4861篇 |
2012年 | 1781篇 |
2011年 | 1938篇 |
2010年 | 1137篇 |
2009年 | 1144篇 |
2008年 | 1749篇 |
2007年 | 1712篇 |
2006年 | 1519篇 |
2005年 | 1388篇 |
2004年 | 1355篇 |
2003年 | 1207篇 |
2002年 | 1267篇 |
2001年 | 1789篇 |
2000年 | 1700篇 |
1999年 | 1324篇 |
1998年 | 713篇 |
1997年 | 608篇 |
1996年 | 554篇 |
1995年 | 553篇 |
1994年 | 540篇 |
1993年 | 555篇 |
1992年 | 1045篇 |
1991年 | 993篇 |
1990年 | 965篇 |
1989年 | 886篇 |
1988年 | 841篇 |
1987年 | 841篇 |
1986年 | 833篇 |
1985年 | 890篇 |
1984年 | 694篇 |
1983年 | 617篇 |
1982年 | 505篇 |
1979年 | 683篇 |
1978年 | 526篇 |
1976年 | 480篇 |
1975年 | 625篇 |
1974年 | 660篇 |
1973年 | 626篇 |
1972年 | 560篇 |
1971年 | 501篇 |
1968年 | 567篇 |
1967年 | 567篇 |
1966年 | 501篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
On the nature of intersensory facilitation of reaction time 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
982.
Although there were no significant differences in the single support phase of gait for 6 trainable mentally retarded boys (Mean age 9.6 yr.) and 6 nonretarded boys (Mean age 9.5 yr.), total gait time and time in double support were significantly different for the two groups, supporting previous research which showed gait was was deficient in trainable mentally retarded individuals. 相似文献
983.
A A Ashby 《Perceptual and motor skills》1983,57(2):649-650
The present experiment was designed to examine age-related characteristics of short-term retention of kinesthetic movement information. 20 children from each age group (6, 8, 10 yr.) were tested for recall of kinesthetic end-location. The results indicate that 6- and 8-yr.-old children became less accurate and more variable in recall performance following a delay interval. However, 10-yr.-old children were capable of maintaining performance accuracy during a delay interval. 相似文献
984.
Kenneth L. Lichstein Timothy J. Hoelscher Terry L. Eakin Rene Nickel 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1983,5(2):111-118
We have previously reported on a sleep assessment device (SAD) that addresses shortcomings inherent in self-report sleep data and polysomnography. The SAD produces a sleep record by tape recording subjects' verbal responses to auditory cues spaced at fixed intervals throughout the night. In prior studies, the SAD proved to be a reliable and valid assessment tool according to electroencephalographic comparisons. Herein, we report on the unassisted home use of the SAD by eight normal volunteers and four bedmates. Subjects and bedmates completed sleep questionnaires for 12 nights and subjects also employed the SAD the last 6 nights. Comparisons of subjects and bedmates self-report sleep data before and during SAD use indicated that the SAD did not disrupt sleep. Subjects were able to self-administer successfully the SAD in their homes. SAD data closely matched subjects' self-report data. The present study found the SAD to be an accurate, unobtrusive, and easily utilized empirical measure of sleep in the natural environment. In so doing, it extends the demonstrated reliability and validity of the SAD from the sleep laboratory to the home with normal sleepers. 相似文献
985.
Steven W. Zucker Kent A. Stevens Peter Sander 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1983,34(6):513-522
The Gestalt studies demonstrated the tendency to visually organize dots on the basis of similarity, proximity, and global properties such as closure, good continuation, and symmetry. The particular organization imposed on a collection of dots is thus determined by many factors, some local, some global. We discuss computational reasons for expecting the initial stages of grouping to be achieved by processes with purely local support. In the case of dot patterns, the expectation is that neighboring dots are grouped as a function of proximity and similarity of contrast, by processes that are independent of the overall organization and the various global factors. We describe experiments that suggest a purely local relationship between proximity and brightness similarity in perceptual grouping. 相似文献
986.
987.
Many basic psychophysical functions offer promise as clinical tests of vision. Here, we discuss problems that one encounters in the clinical setting, how one identifies a psychophysical test for potential clinical development, and an orderly approach to development of suitable test paradigms. Parameters are selected which are relatively insensitive to variables encountered in the field (clinic) in a normal population, but which are sensitive to changes in the response system being studied. Initial data on two hyperacuity tests are presented. These tests are adaptations of hyperacuity paradigms (Westheimer, 1979) to a clinical environment. This particular test set offers promise because it exhibits a unique threshold which is dependent upon neural data processing and is relatively independent of retinal image degradation. 相似文献
988.
The latency of reading a single word is increased by 20 to 40 msec if another object is present in the display. The delay is affected by the spatial organization of the display: a colored frame causes less delay when it surrounds the word than when it is shown on the opposite side of fixation. A small gap in the frame is also more efficiently detected as a secondary task when the frame is around the word than when the two are spatially separate. The advantage of integrated over separate presentation suggests that a “filtering cost” is incurred when two distinct perceptual objects compete for attention. Attention in filtering tasks operates on perceptually distinct objects rather than on nodes in a semantic network. 相似文献
989.
A. S. Karpenko 《Studia Logica》1983,42(2-3):179-185
In this note we prove that some familiar systems of finitely many-valued logics havefactor semantics, and establish necessary conditions for a system of many-valued logic having semantics of this kind. 相似文献
990.