全文获取类型
收费全文 | 370篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
371篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 4篇 |
2013年 | 39篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 9篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 12篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 18篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有371条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
222.
Previous theoretical and empirical work indicates that intentional changes in a bimanual coordination pattern depends on the stability of the bimanual coordination pattern (Kelso, Schotz, & Schöner, 1988; Scholz & Kelso, 1990). The present experiments retest this notion when online Lissajous displays are provided. Switching to and from in-phase and antiphase and to and from 90° and 270° were tested in Experiment 1. Participants were able to very effectively produce the 180°, 90°, and 270° coordination patterns although performance of the in-phase coordination task was even more stable. The data indicated that switching to in-phase from antiphase was more rapid than vice versa and that switching times between 90° to 270° were similar. Experiment 2 investigated switching between 1:2 and 2:1 bimanual coordination patterns. The results indicated that switching time was similar between the 2:1 and 1:2 coordination tasks and that increases in stability over practice resulted in additional decreases in switching times. This provides additional evidence that the attractor landscape is fundamentally different dependent on the type of information provided the performer. What remains to be done is to reconcile these results with the various theories/perspectives currently used to describe and explain bimanual coordination. 相似文献
223.
Taylor Kennedy Tom Cariveau Kathryn Grelck Alexandria Brown Delanie F. Platt Paige Ellington 《Behavioral Interventions》2024,39(2):e1992
Matching-to-sample arrangements are commonly used to teach conditional discriminations. In these arrangements, instructors must systematically arrange instruction to ensure that a learner's response comes under the intended sources of stimulus control. Given the multitude of instructional considerations, the instructors' procedural fidelity has been a significant concern. Recently, LeBlanc et al. found that brief training and access to enhanced data sheets produced high levels of fidelity with experienced service providers. The current study extended LeBlanc et al. by examining the effects of a similar training on the fidelity and instructional pacing by participants with and without previous experience. The participants' performance was also compared when using a flashcard or binder (i.e., printed) arrays and relative to a tablet-delivered instructional program. High levels of fidelity were observed following training, although pacing was slow. Slight differences in performance were observed across comparison arrays; nevertheless, the tablet-based program outperformed instructors. 相似文献
224.
225.
Hazel I. Blythe Jonathan H. Dickins Colin R. Kennedy Simon P. Liversedge 《Developmental science》2018,21(5)
There has been considerable variability within the literature concerning the extent to which deaf/hard of hearing individuals are able to process phonological codes during reading. Two experiments are reported in which participants’ eye movements were recorded as they read sentences containing correctly spelled words (e.g., church), pseudohomophones (e.g., cherch), and spelling controls (e.g., charch). We examined both foveal processing and parafoveal pre‐processing of phonology for three participant groups—teenagers with permanent childhood hearing loss (PCHL), chronological age‐matched controls, and reading age‐matched controls. The teenagers with PCHL showed a pseudohomophone advantage from both directly fixated words and parafoveal preview, similar to their hearing peers. These data provide strong evidence for phonological recoding during silent reading in teenagers with PCHL. 相似文献
226.
227.
The main sources of stress reported by 423 Australian final-year high school students using the Academic Stress Questionnaire were school-related as expected. The highest sources of this stress were examinations and outcomes, too much to do, worry over future, making choices about career, studying for examinations, amount to learn, need to do well imposed by others, and self-imposed need to do well. 相似文献
228.
229.
Current debate exists regarding the timing of reconstructive surgery following mastectomy for breast cancer, with research pointing in favour of immediate reconstruction. This cross-sectional study sought to compare the psychological outcome of breast cancer treatment in women who had either received mastectomy and immediate reconstruction using autogenous tissue (n?=?30), or mastectomy alone (n?=?34), and also determine adjustment factors in this population as a whole. Participants completed measures of depression, anxiety, body image, self-esteem, coping and perceived social support at a time point 3?-?15 months after initial surgery. No significant differences were revealed between the two groups on any of the outcome measures. Poor body image, low self-esteem, and a tendency to use coping strategies characterized by helpless/hopelessness and anxious preoccupation, rather than fighting spirit, were highly predictive of distress. Reasons for the lack of differences between the two groups are explored. 相似文献
230.
Nine experiments examined the formation of an abstract category representation for the spatial relation between by 6- to 10-month-old infants. The experiments determined that 9- to 10-month-olds, but not 6- to 7-month-olds, could form an abstract category representation for between when performing in an object-variation version of the between categorization task. The results also demonstrated that 6- to 7-month-olds could form category representations for between in the object-variation version of the between categorization task but that such representations were specific to the particular objects presented. The evidence confirms that representations for different spatial relations emerge at different points during development, and suggests that each representation undergoes its own period of development from concrete to abstract. 相似文献