全文获取类型
收费全文 | 482篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
493篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 65篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 33篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有493条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
291.
Analogical learning has long been seen as a powerful way of extending the reach of one’s knowledge. We present the domain transfer via analogy (DTA) method for learning new domain theories via cross-domain analogy. Our model uses analogies between pairs of textbook example problems, or worked solutions, to create a domain mapping between a familiar and a new domain. This mapping allows us to initialize a new domain theory. After this initialization, another analogy is made between the domain theories themselves, providing additional conjectures about the new domain. We present two experiments in which our model learns rotational kinematics by an analogy with translational kinematics, and vice versa. These learning rates outperform those from a version of the system that is incrementally given the correct domain theory. 相似文献
292.
Methods for discriminant analysis were compared with respect to classification accuracy under nonnormality through Monte Carlo
simulation. The methods compared were linear discriminant analyses based both on raw scores and on ranks; linear logistic
discrimination; and mixture discriminant analysis. Linear discriminant analysis and linear logistic discrimination were suboptimal
in a number of scenarios with skewed predictors. Linear discriminant analysis based on ranks yielded the highest rates of
classification accuracy in only a limited number of situations and did not produce a practically important advantage over
competing methods. Mixture discriminant analysis, with a relatively small number of components in each group, attained relatively
high rates of classification accuracy and was most useful for conditions in which skewed predictors had relatively small values
of kurtosis. 相似文献
293.
Timothy D. Sweeny Marcia Grabowecky Satoru Suzuki Ken A. Paller 《Consciousness and cognition》2009,18(4):929-938
Unconscious processing of stimuli with emotional content can bias affective judgments. Is this subliminal affective priming merely a transient phenomenon manifested in fleeting perceptual changes, or are long-lasting effects also induced? To address this question, we investigated memory for surprise faces 24 h after they had been shown with 30-ms fearful, happy, or neutral faces. Surprise faces subliminally primed by happy faces were initially rated as more positive, and were later remembered better, than those primed by fearful or neutral faces. Participants likely to have processed primes supraliminally did not respond differentially as a function of expression. These results converge with findings showing memory advantages with happy expressions, though here the expressions were displayed on the face of a different person, perceived subliminally, and not present at test. We conclude that behavioral biases induced by masked emotional expressions are not ephemeral, but rather can last at least 24 h. 相似文献
294.
Ken Aizawa 《Synthese》2009,167(3):493-510
One trend in recent work on topic of the multiple realization of psychological properties has been an emphasis on greater
sensitivity to actual science and greater clarity regarding the metaphysics of realization and multiple realization. One contribution
to this trend is Bechtel and Mundale’s examination of the implications of brain mapping for multiple realization. Where Bechtel
and Mundale argue that studies of brain mapping undermine claims about the multiple realization, this paper challenges that
argument. 相似文献
295.
296.
Niall Galbraith Ken Manktelow Neil Morris 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2008,99(1):29-44
Previous studies (e.g. Moller & Husby, 2000 ; Blackwood et al., 2004 ) have revealed that delusional thinking is accompanied by an exaggerated focus upon the self and upon stimuli that are perceived to be related to the self. The objective was to examine whether those high in subclinical delusional ideation exhibit a heightened tendency for self‐reference. Using a mixed design, healthy individuals, classified into high‐ and low‐scoring groups on the Peters et al. Delusions Inventory ( Peters, Day, & Garety, 1996 ), were compared on everyday reasoning tasks across three experiments. High‐PDI scorers, in contrast to the low‐PDI group, rated self‐referent objections to everyday arguments as stronger than other‐referent objections and formulated more self‐referent assertion‐based objections to everyday arguments. The findings support the notion that subclinical delusional ideation is linked to a self‐reference bias, which is evident in the sort of everyday thinking that people engage in when forming or evaluating their beliefs and which may contribute to delusion formation. 相似文献
297.
The aim of this study was to assess the impact of the Newfoundland and Labrador groundfish moratorium on the health and social wellness of two communities severely affected by the industry collapse. A qualitative study was conducted involving individual and group interviews with residents from communities in which differences (as indexed primarily by economic and demographic statistics) were observed. Using the concept of social capital and its associated themes of help and support, trust, leadership, and civic engagement, it was observed that the ‘high crisis community’ also demonstrated negative alterations in social and political characteristics which may have compromised its capacity to cope with the crisis and translated into detriments in resident wellness. Among a variety of identified challenges, out‐migration appeared to be the greatest threat as it has translated into an assortment of negative realities. The utility of social capital as a framework for understanding community crises is also discussed. 相似文献
298.
299.
It has been widely claimed that attention and awareness are doubly dissociable and that there is no causal relation between them. In support of this view are numerous claims of attention without awareness, and awareness without attention. Although there is evidence that attention can operate on or be drawn to unconscious stimuli, various recent findings demonstrate that there is no empirical support for awareness without attention. To properly test for awareness without attention, we propose that a stimulus be studied using a battery of tests based on diverse, mainstream paradigms from the current attention literature. When this type of analysis is performed, the evidence is fully consistent with a model in which attention is necessary, but not sufficient, for awareness. 相似文献
300.
Subthreshold summation between physical target lines and illusory contours induced by edges such as those produced in the Kanizsa illusion has been reported in previous studies. Here, we investigated the ability of line-induced illusory contours, using Ehrenstein figures, to produce similar subthreshold summation. In the first experiment, three stimulus conditions were presented. The target line was superimposed on the illusory contour of a four-arm Ehrenstein figure, or the target was presented between two dots (which replaced the arms of the Ehrenstein figure), or the target was presented on an otherwise blank screen (control). Detection of the target line was significantly worse when presented on the illusory contour (on the Ehrenstein figure) than when presented between two dots. This result was consistent for both curved and straight target lines, as well as for a 100 ms presentation duration and unlimited presentation duration. Performance was worst in the control condition. The results for the three stimulus conditions were replicated in a second experiment in which an eight-arm Ehrenstein figure was used to produce a stronger and less ambiguous illusory contour. In the third experiment, the target was either superimposed on the illusory contour, or was located across the central gap (illusory surface) of the Ehrenstein figure, collinear with two arms of the figure. As in the first two experiments, the target was either presented on the Ehrenstein figure, or between dots, or on a blank screen. Detection was better in the dot condition than in the Ehrenstein condition, regardless of whether the target was presented on the illusory contour or collinear with the arms of the Ehrenstein figure. These three experiments demonstrate the ability of reduced spatial uncertainty to facilitate the detection of a target line, but do not provide any evidence for subthreshold summation between a physical target line and the illusory contours produced by an Ehrenstein figure. The incongruence of these results with previous findings on Kanizsa figures is discussed. 相似文献