全文获取类型
收费全文 | 482篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
493篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 65篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 29篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 33篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有493条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
112.
113.
Veronica J. Dark Ken Benson 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1991,43(1):53-78
Three experiments examined both the impact of semantic analysis of 50-msec, masked visual primes on a target response and the impact of semantic analysis of the target on a prime response. The first two experiments used a prime-target interval of 1000 msec. In Experiment 1, subjects reported the identity of each prime: (a) after a lexical decision about the target, (b) both before and after a lexical decision, or (c) after a target detection response. Prime report after both types of target response showed retroactive priming in which report was facilitated by related targets and inhibited by unrelated targets. Analyses of lexical decision latency and accuracy conditionalized on prime report showed that semantic priming was restricted to reported related primes. In Experiment 2, subjects made no overt response to the primes. Priming was conditionalized on recognition of the primes on a subsequent test. The pattern was the same as Experiment 1: There was priming only for recognized primes; recognition memory showed a pattern consistent with retroactive priming. Experiment 3 also conditionalized priming on recognition performance but used a prime-target interval of only 250 msec. Again, semantic priming was found only for recognized primes, and recognition memory revealed retroactive priming. Retroactive priming indicates an interdependency between prime and target processing that needs to be incorporated into models of semantic priming. 相似文献
114.
Ken Corbett Ph.D. 《Psychoanalytic Dialogues》2013,23(4):457-474
The relational reach and psychotherapeutic action of comedy are examined in the opening phase of treatment with a man who had an early history of neglect and consequent neurotic isolation. Through the examination of a series of comedic enactments that cracked into the beginning phase of this treatment, comedy's animating force to defy neurotic gravity and aid in the reanimation of primal density is considered. The ways in which laughter opened into a maternal erotic transference—countertransference is further detailed, in conjunction with reflection aimed at the aggression of this comedic bond, including countertransference hate. 相似文献
115.
Although very little visual information is explicitly retained across views, some continuity of processing is afforded by implicit visual memory traces of previous views. These memory traces interact with attentional mechanisms to guide eye movements, cognition, and action. Two different memory mechanisms are described here. First, the deployments of focal attention and eye movements are facilitated towards recently attended features and locations (priming of popout). Second, attention is guided by implicit memory traces of specific visual contexts experienced in the past (contextual cueing). Compared to the visual memory tapped by change blindness tasks, the implicit memory mechanisms of priming of popout and contextual cueing do not require conscious intervention and may exhibit greater memory capacity, longer durability, and higher discriminability. Thus, these implicit traces of past views guide attention and eye movements to allow for effective access (indexing) to a scene's details, hence providing context and continuity to ongoing interactions with the perceptual world. 相似文献
116.
Ken J. Rotenberg Serena Petrocchi Flavia Lecciso Antonella Marchetti 《Infant and child development》2015,24(2):206-214
The study examined the relation between children's trust beliefs and Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities. A sample of 168 Italian children (M = 9 years–6 months, SD = 7 months) were administered the Italian Children's Generalized Trust Beliefs (ICGTB) Scale, two Second‐Order False Belief ToM measures, and an Advanced ToM measure. As expected, the ICGTB scale demonstrated: (1) validity by its three factor structure and (2) reliability by exhibiting acceptable internal consistency and test–retest stability. As expected, the children's emotional trust beliefs in others were associated with both second‐order false belief ToM ability and advanced ToM ability. These relations were not attributable to verbal ability. The findings are discussed with respect to the relations among children's emotional trust beliefs, personal disclosure, and quality of attachment. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
117.
118.
Declan Fay Alan Carr Ken O'Reilly Paul Cahill Barbara Dooley Suzanne Guerin Peter Stratton 《Journal of Family Therapy》2013,35(1):24-42
In this study a 29‐item version of the systemic clinical outcome and routine evaluation (SCORE), which contained all items from the SCORE‐15 and SCORE‐28, was used to develop norms for both the 15 and the 28 versions of the SCORE from the same sample. In a random digit dialling telephone survey, a stratified national random sample of 403 adults living in the Republic of Ireland and Northern Ireland completed the SCORE and brief measures of family and personal adjustment. Using receiver operating characteristic curve analyses, cut‐off points for the SCORE‐28 and 15 were found to identify families of children with significant emotional and behavioural problems. We also established 90th percentile points and percentages of cases falling above each scale point for both versions of the SCORE. Confirmatory factor analyses showed that, for both versions of the SCORE, the data fit the three factor solutions found in previous studies. The SCORE scales also had significant correlations with measures of family, parental and child adjustment, and negligible correlations with socioeconomic variables and social desirability response set. 相似文献
119.
The present study aimed to investigate the prevalence of fatigue and determine factors associated with fatigue in female medical personnel. Based on a cross-sectional study, a total of 1608 female medical personnel at 54 hospitals in Zhuhai, China were recruited by a multistage stratified cluster sampling method. The Symptoms Checklist-90-Revised and Chalder Fatigue Scale were used to assess psychiatric symptoms and fatigue, respectively. Data regarding demographic, health, and work related variables were also collected. Multivariate logistic regression model was constructed to determine the influencing factors of fatigue. Approximately 83% of participants had experienced fatigue in the past week. The risk of fatigue was higher in aged 30–39 years old than older or younger participants; Longer sleeping time predicted a lower prevalence of fatigue (OR = .35), while tense physician-patient relationship predicted a higher prevalence of fatigue (OR = 1.77). Depression (OR = 1.76) and anxiety (OR = 1.96) were found related to fatigue. Additionally, fatigue was associated with marital status, occupation, health related factors (exercise, regular diet, and health status), and work related factors (hospital rank and turnover intention). These study findings might facilitate development and implementation of targeted interventions and preventive measures. 相似文献
120.
Whether bullying in schools is increasing, as is widely believed, was investigated drawing upon empirical studies undertaken
in a wide range of countries in which findings had been published describing its prevalence at different points in time between
1990 and 2009. Results do not support the view that reported bullying in general has increased during this period; in fact,
a significant decrease in bullying has been reported in many countries. However, there are some indications that cyber bullying,
as opposed to traditional bullying, has increased, at least during some of this period. The reported decreases in the prevalence
of school bullying are consistent with reports of significant but small reductions in peer victimisation following the implementation
of anti-bullying programs in schools world-wide. 相似文献