全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2884篇 |
免费 | 36篇 |
专业分类
2920篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 23篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 73篇 |
2018年 | 100篇 |
2017年 | 89篇 |
2016年 | 91篇 |
2015年 | 60篇 |
2014年 | 90篇 |
2013年 | 295篇 |
2012年 | 130篇 |
2011年 | 124篇 |
2010年 | 77篇 |
2009年 | 88篇 |
2008年 | 128篇 |
2007年 | 122篇 |
2006年 | 89篇 |
2005年 | 70篇 |
2004年 | 85篇 |
2003年 | 90篇 |
2002年 | 95篇 |
2001年 | 45篇 |
2000年 | 42篇 |
1999年 | 38篇 |
1998年 | 44篇 |
1997年 | 34篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 32篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 25篇 |
1978年 | 27篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 33篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1972年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有2920条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
Models of racial attitudes traditionally have assumed that individual differences in the strength of underlying, 'implicit' associations between racial categories and stereotypical traits are the primary determinant of the expression of race bias. Thus, individual differences in performance on laboratory tasks designed to assess implicit race bias tend to be interpreted in terms of association strength. Here, we argue that such associations tell only part of the story, and probably the least interesting part. We posit that response conflict and its regulation are critical to understanding the need for control, and that affect-related processes help to determine the extent to which control resources will be implemented to overcome biased associations. We present data from a number of recent behavioral and psychophysiological studies in support of this idea, as well as conceptual accounts that point toward a model of race bias regulation that depends upon processes identified as important for regulation of thought, affect and action more generally. 相似文献
152.
The pathological left-handedness syndrome 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
A clinical syndrome of pathological left-handedness (PLH) is proposed to identify the pattern of correlative changes in lateral development associated with early brain injury in some manifest left-handers. This syndrome is believed to be caused by a hemispheric lesion that is predominantly left-sided (or bilateral asymmetric), which onsets before Age 6, and which encroaches upon the critical speech zones of the frontotemporal/frontoparietal cortex. The pattern of changes may include any or all of the following features: shifts in manual dominance, trophic changes in the extremities, transfer of hemispheric speech, and/or intrahemispheric reorganization of visuospatial cognitive functions. Although some of these correlates of PLH have long been known, they have not been recognized as an interrelated pattern of traits that constitute a clinical syndrome. Identification of these individuals, all manifest left-handers, will be shown to have implications for diagnosis/remediation and for models of recovery of function. 相似文献
153.
154.
Kelly M. Wolenberg John D. Yoon Kenneth A. Rasinski Farr A. Curlin 《Journal of religion and health》2013,52(4):1051-1065
This study surveyed 1,156 practicing US physicians to examine the relationship between physicians’ religious characteristics and their approaches to artificial nutrition and hydration (ANH). Forty percent of physicians believed that unless a patient is imminently dying, the patient should always receive nutrition and fluids; 75 % believed that it is ethically permissible for doctors to withdraw ANH. The least religious physicians were less likely to oppose withholding or withdrawing ANH. Compared to non-evangelical Protestant physicians, Jews and Muslims were significantly more likely to oppose withholding ANH, and Muslims were significantly more likely to oppose withdrawing ANH. 相似文献
155.
Henry E. Heffner Gimseong Koay Evan M. Hill Rickye S. Heffner 《Behavior research methods》2013,45(2):383-392
Although the domestic pigeon is commonly used in learning experiments, it is a notoriously difficult subject in auditory psychophysical experiments, even those in which it need only respond when it detects a sound. This is because pigeons tend to respond in the absence of sound—that is, they have a high false-positive rate—which makes it difficult to determine a pigeon’s audiogram. However, false positives are easily controlled in the method of conditioned suppression/avoidance, in which a pigeon is trained to peck a key to obtain food and to stop pecking whenever it detects a sound that signals impending electric shock. Here, we describe how to determine psychophysical thresholds in pigeons using a method of conditioned suppression in which avoidable shock is delivered through a bead chain wrapped around the base of a pigeon’s wings. The resulting audiogram spans the range from 2 to 8000 Hz; it falls approximately in the middle of the distribution of previous pigeon audiograms and supports the finding of Kreithen and Quine (Journal of Comparative Physiology 129:1–4, 1979) that pigeons hear infrasound. 相似文献
156.
The testing effect is the finding that retrieval practice can enhance recall on future tests. One unanswered question is whether first-test response mode (writing or speaking the answer) affects final-test performance (and whether final-test response mode itself matters). An additional unsettled issue is whether written and oral recall lead to differences in the amount recalled. In three experiments, we examined these issues: whether subjects can recall more via typing or speaking; whether typing or speaking answers on a first test can lead to better final-test performance (and whether an interaction occurs with final-test response mode) and whether any form of overt response leads to better final-test performance as compared to covert retrieval (thinking of the answer but not producing it). Subjects studied paired associates; took a first test by typing, speaking, or thinking about responses; and then took a second test in which the answers were either spoken or typed. The results revealed few differences between typing and speaking during recall, and no difference in the size of the testing effect on the second test. Furthermore, an initial covert retrieval yielded roughly the same benefit to future test performance as did overt retrieval. Thus, the testing effect was quite robust across these manipulations. The practical implication for learning is that covert retrieval provides as much benefit to later retention as does overt retrieval and that both can be effective study strategies. 相似文献
157.
Kelly Trogdon 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2013,91(3):555-558
Two competing views in sparse ontology are monism and pluralism. In Trogdon 2009 I propose an account of intrinsicality that I argue is both compatible with monism and pluralism and independently plausible. Skiles 2009 argues that my account fails on both fronts. In this note I respond to his two objections. 相似文献
158.
159.
Functional tool usage is a critical aspect of our daily lives. Not only must we know which tools to use for a specific action goal, we must also know how to manipulate those tools in meaningful way to achieve the goal of the action. The purpose of this study was to identify the regions of the brain critical to supporting the process of understanding errors in tool manipulation. Using fMRI, neural activations were recorded while subjects were presented with images demonstrating typical action scenes (screwdriver used on a screw), but with the tool being manipulated either correctly (screwdriver held by handle) or incorrectly (screwdriver held by bit rather than handle). Activations in fMRI for identifying correct over incorrect tool manipulation were seen along the canonical parietofrontal action network, while activations for identifying incorrect over correct tool manipulation were primarily seen at superior temporal areas and insula. We expand our hypotheses about ventral brain networks identifying contextual error to further suggest mechanisms for understanding functional tool actions, which collectively we regard as functional affordances. This proposes a fundamental role for ventral brain areas in functional action understanding. 相似文献
160.