全文获取类型
收费全文 | 43505篇 |
免费 | 461篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
43972篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 276篇 |
2018年 | 3766篇 |
2017年 | 3116篇 |
2016年 | 2613篇 |
2015年 | 527篇 |
2014年 | 460篇 |
2013年 | 1981篇 |
2012年 | 1234篇 |
2011年 | 3020篇 |
2010年 | 2868篇 |
2009年 | 1826篇 |
2008年 | 2194篇 |
2007年 | 2691篇 |
2006年 | 539篇 |
2005年 | 695篇 |
2004年 | 633篇 |
2003年 | 565篇 |
2002年 | 535篇 |
2001年 | 806篇 |
2000年 | 786篇 |
1999年 | 579篇 |
1998年 | 268篇 |
1997年 | 231篇 |
1996年 | 250篇 |
1995年 | 218篇 |
1992年 | 467篇 |
1991年 | 455篇 |
1990年 | 444篇 |
1989年 | 416篇 |
1988年 | 429篇 |
1987年 | 393篇 |
1986年 | 400篇 |
1985年 | 402篇 |
1984年 | 322篇 |
1983年 | 294篇 |
1982年 | 219篇 |
1981年 | 224篇 |
1979年 | 361篇 |
1978年 | 272篇 |
1977年 | 225篇 |
1975年 | 293篇 |
1974年 | 367篇 |
1973年 | 372篇 |
1972年 | 295篇 |
1971年 | 285篇 |
1970年 | 286篇 |
1969年 | 256篇 |
1968年 | 354篇 |
1967年 | 308篇 |
1966年 | 286篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
4.
Research in both laboratory and field settings has suggested a link between thermal stress and violent behavior, and both linear and curvilinear models have been investigated. A dearth of field studies prompted the analysis reported here, which is based on data for some 10,000 aggravated assaults occurring the City of Dallas in 1980 (a summer of severe heat stress) and 1981. This analysis replicates and extends certain aspects of recent work by Anderson and Anderson (1984) relating to the so-called linear and curvilinear hypotheses. Thermal stress is measured in two ways: a Discomfort Index (DI), which takes into account the influence of humidity acting in concert with temperature, and ambient temperature. Regression analyses were performed in two stages. In the first, data for all neighborhoods and all days of the study period were combined into ambient temperature and DI models. At the second stage, models differentiated between the three levels of neighborhood socioeconomic status. With weekend controlled, DI and ambient temperature were significant independent variables in the ‘overall’ model and in medium and low status neighborhoods. However, when linear effects were controlled, the curvilinear measures were never significant. The analysis generally tended to confirm Anderson and Anderson's suggestion that a reduction of aggression with increasing temperature does not appear to occur within the normal range of temperatures. This analysis further suggested that the hypothesized curvilinear effect is weak, if not entirely absent, even during conditions of extreme heat. 相似文献
5.
6.
We studied the effect of different postoperative times on the behavioral recovery following brain implants. Adult male rats received cortical tissue grafts 2 weeks after aspiration of the medial frontal cortex. Either 2 (Immediate Group) or 28 (Delay Group) days after grafting, the performance of these rats on a behavioral battery, comprising the Morris water maze task, forepaw use, and grooming, was compared to that of rats with similar lesions and postoperative recovery times but no grafts. Rats tested immediately after receiving implants performed better on the spatial navigation task than rats with similar lesions but no grafts. This improvement, however, was less than that shown by rats with lesions but no grafts permitted to recover for 28 days before testing. In contrast, in the Delay Group, rats with grafts were more greatly impaired than were their operated controls. Neither lesions nor grafts affected grooming although the Immediate Group with grafts were significantly more impaired in using their forepaws during feeding than were any of the other groups. These results lead us to conclude that differing postoperative recovery times and task requirements may account for some of the inconsistent results of the influence of brain grafts on behavioral recovery reported in the literature. We also conclude that cortical tissue implants can have two effects with different time courses and opposite net behavioral effects. 相似文献
7.
8.
Past research in referential communication has indicated normally developing children show developmental progression in ability to communicate a specific referent to a listener. In one paradigm subjects were given lists of word-pairs in which one member of each pair was designated as the referent. It was shown that communicating about referents found in word-pairs associated in some way was more difficult than communicating about referents in dissimilar word-pairs. The present study extended this methodology to learning-disabled children. Learning-disabled, language-learning-disabled, and normally achieving children were asked to communicate about 30 pictured referents on three different tasks. On Tasks 1 and 2 each subject was asked to give a clue for the referent that would distinguish it from the other picture. Stimuli for Task 1 were 30 pairs of pictures that were related in some way and the stimuli for Task 2 were 30 pairs of unrelated pictures. Task 3 required the subjects to evaluate the adequacy of the examiner's clues for Task 1 stimuli. The disabled subjects were matched to the normally achieving subjects on the basis of receptive vocabulary age. Few differences were noted among the groups' performances on these referential communication tasks. Implications include the importance of vocabulary and concept development to referential communication. 相似文献
9.
S W?sser W Handrick F B Spencker W Braun 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(2):95-101
Observations on three children with neurolisteriosis (one case of meningitis, two cases of meningoencephalitis, each Serovar 4 b), show that even after the neonatal period, listeriosis must not be ignored in the process of diagnosis and therapy. It is the bacteriological examination of the cerebrospinal fluid, together with the blood culture, and not clinical symptoms and serology that guarantee a timely diagnosis and therapy (ampicillin and gentamicin). 相似文献
10.