全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2864篇 |
免费 | 135篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3000篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 47篇 |
2019年 | 58篇 |
2018年 | 81篇 |
2017年 | 84篇 |
2016年 | 116篇 |
2015年 | 62篇 |
2014年 | 92篇 |
2013年 | 344篇 |
2012年 | 150篇 |
2011年 | 158篇 |
2010年 | 98篇 |
2009年 | 87篇 |
2008年 | 141篇 |
2007年 | 119篇 |
2006年 | 110篇 |
2005年 | 94篇 |
2004年 | 100篇 |
2003年 | 93篇 |
2002年 | 71篇 |
2001年 | 65篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 41篇 |
1994年 | 27篇 |
1993年 | 33篇 |
1992年 | 32篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 32篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 23篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 18篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 11篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有3000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Rudd MD Berman AL Joiner TE Nock MK Silverman MM Mandrusiak M Van Orden K Witte T 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2006,36(3):255-262
The current article addresses the issue of warning signs for suicide, attempting to differentiate the construct from risk factors. In accordance with the characteristic features discussed, a consensus set of warning signs identified by the American Association of Suicidology working group are presented, along with a discussion of relevant clinical and research applications. 相似文献
62.
Eric Rassin Jannie Van Der Sleen Adri G. Van Amelsvoort Eveline Buttinger 《Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling》2006,3(3):157-163
Fifty children, aged between 4 and 7 years, underwent a sequential photo line‐up procedure in order to identify a man they had met the previous day. Half of the participants engaged in a pre‐procedure gameplay with the female interviewer. It was hypothesised that the game would stimulate rapport building and would ultimately result in an increased performance during the line‐up procedure. The beneficial effect was indeed observed, but only for the male participants. This finding suggests that gameplay may be a fruitful strategy to decrease child witness discomfort and to increase participation. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
63.
Anna M. L. Van Brakel Peter Muris 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2006,28(2):79-84
The behavioral inhibition scale (BIS) is a brief rating scale for measuring behavioral inhibition in children. The present
study examined the test–retest stability of the BIS in a sample of 7–12-year-olds consisting of inhibited and non-inhibited
children (N = 83). Results demonstrated that the BIS scores were fairly stable over a 2-year period, with a test–retest correlation of
.77. Further, the BIS scores of inhibited children significantly increased over the 2-year period, whereas those of the non-inhibited
comparison group significantly decreased. Finally, the BIS was in a theoretically meaningful way associated with temperamental
traits as measured by the Rothbart scales. Altogether, these results provide support for the reliability and validity of the
BIS. 相似文献
64.
In an information state where various agents have both factual knowledge and knowledge about each other, announcements can
be made that change the state of information. Such informative announcements can have the curious property that they become
false because they are announced. The most typical example of that is ‘fact p is true and you don’t know that’, after which you know that p, which entails the negation of the announcement formula. The announcement of such a formula in a given information state
is called an unsuccessful update. A successful formula is a formula that always becomes common knowledge after being announced.
Analysis of information systems and ‘philosophical puzzles’ reveals a growing number of dynamic phenomena that can be described
or explained by unsuccessful updates. This increases our understanding of such philosophical problems. We also investigate
the syntactic characterization of the successful formulas.
An erratum to this article is available at . 相似文献
65.
66.
67.
Rob W. Holland Bas Verplanken Ad Van Knippenberg 《European journal of social psychology》2002,32(6):869-876
This study investigated the role of attitude strength as a moderator variable with regard to the direction of the relation between attitudes and behavior. The hypothesis was tested that strong attitudes guide behavior, whereas weak attitudes follow behavior in accordance with self‐perception principles. The study (N = 106) consisted of two sessions. In session 1, attitudes and attitude strength (certainty, importance, centrality) towards Greenpeace were measured. One week later, participants returned to the laboratory (session 2) and were given the opportunity to donate money to Greenpeace. After the participants' decision to donate money or not, attitudes towards Greenpeace were measured again. The results were consistent with the predictions. First, strong attitudes were more predictive of donation behavior than weak attitudes. Moreover, session 2 attitudes of weak attitude participants were influenced by their donation behavior, whereas no such effect was found among strong attitude participants. Finally, strong attitudes were also found to be more stable over time than weak attitudes. The results provide a complete overview of the moderating role of attitude strength with regard to the bi‐directional attitude‐behavior relationship. Results are discussed in the light of attitude retrieval versus attitude‐construction processes. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
68.
69.
Scahill L Lombroso PJ Mack G Van Wattum PJ Zhang H Vitale A Leckman JF 《Perceptual and motor skills》2001,92(2):419-432
The effects of heat on tic symptoms were studied in a sample of 78 adults with Tourette syndrome. 62 men and 16 women completed a survey concerning the type, onset, and course of their tics. 10 adult male subjects also participated in a thermal challenge during which ambient temperature was raised from 22 degrees C to 35 degrees C following a control period. Of the 78, 24% or 19 reported increased tics upon exposure to heat. Compared to the remaining 59 subjects, there were no differences in sex distribution, current age, or overall course of illness. In the thermal challenge, there was general increase in tics that was correlated with sweat rate (r = .55, p = .001). This effect was prominent in 5 of 10 subjects (rs = .29 to .63). There were no mean differences in current age, age of onset, or current severity of symptoms between the five subjects of each group. Tic symptoms in a subgroup of patients with Tourette syndrome may be sensitive to heat. Abnormal heat regulation is not a likely explanation for the observed increase in tics. The increase may be due to normal heat-loss mechanisms through dopaminergic pathways. 相似文献
70.
Arend W. A. Van Gemmert Hans-Leo Teulings George E. Stelmach 《Brain and cognition》2001,47(3):504-512
Parkinson's disease (PD) patients often show reductions in writing size (micrographia) as the length of the text they produce increases. The cause for these reductions in stroke size are not well understood. Reductions in stroke size could be associated with either concurrent processing demands that result from the coordination and control of fingers, wrist, and arm during writing and the processing of future words or increased extension of the wrist joint as the execution of the writing progresses to the right across the page, resulting in increased stiffness in the pen-limb system. Parkinson's patients and elderly controls wrote phrases of different lengths with target patterns in various serial positions. When the number of words to be written increased, PD patients reduced their stroke size of the initial target pattern, while the elderly controls did not reduce their stroke size. There was no systematic change in stroke size of the second pattern as function of serial position. This result suggests that PD patients reduce the size of their handwriting strokes when concurrent processing load increases. 相似文献