排序方式: 共有132条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Lauren French Matthew P. Gerrie Maryanne Garry Kazuo Mori 《Behavior research methods》2009,41(4):1224-1232
The MORI technique provides a unique way to research social influences on memory. The technique allows people to watch different
movies on the same screen at the same time without realizing that each of them sees something different. As a result, researchers
can create a situation in which people feel as though they share an experience, but systematic differences are introduced
into their memories, and the effect of those differences can be tracked through a discussion. Despite its methodological advances,
the MORI technique has been met with criticism, mostly because reviewers are worried that the MORI technique might not completely
block the alternate movie version from view, leading people in these studies to see their partner’s version of the movie as
well as their own. We addressed these concerns in two experiments. We found no evidence that subjects noticed the alternate
movie version while watching a movie via the MORI technique (Experiment 1) and no evidence that subjects remembered details
from the alternate movie version (Experiment 2). Taken together, the results provide support for the MORI technique as a valuable
research tool. 相似文献
92.
Mori K 《Perceptual and motor skills》2005,101(3):898-900
32 Japanese participants drew symmetrical and asymmetrical letters on their own foreheads. By observing their drawing strokes, it was judged that 29 of them drew symmetric letters in reverse, that is, as if these were viewed from inside the head. However, 14 of these 29 seemed to have noticed their reversed perception because they drew asymmetric letters intentionally reversed so they would appear correct when viewed from outside. Only three participants drew the letters in a normal way irrespective of the letter symmetry. 相似文献
93.
Squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) were tested using an expectancy violation procedure to assess whether they use an actors gaze direction, signaled by congruent head and eye orientation, to predict subsequent behavior. The monkeys visually habituated to a repeated sequence in which the actor (a familiar human or a puppet) looked at an object and then picked it up, but they did not react strongly when the actor looked at an object but then picked up another object. Capuchin monkeys responses in the puppet condition were slightly more suggestive of expectancy. There was no differential responding to congruent versus incongruent look–touch sequences when familiarization trials were omitted. The weak findings contrast with a strongly positive result previously reported for tamarin monkeys. Additional evidence is required before concluding that behavior prediction based on gaze cues typifies primates; other approaches for studying how they process attention cues are indicated.This revised version was published online in March 2004 with corrections to Fig. 2. The number 1 in the bottom left panel should be a cross and was, moreover, misaligned. 相似文献
94.
Miyata H Ushitani T Adachi I Fujita K 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2006,120(4):358-366
The authors examined how pigeons (Columba livia) perform on 2-dimensional maze tasks on the LCD monitor and whether the pigeons preplan the solution before starting to solve the maze. After training 4 pigeons to move a red square (the target) to a blue square (the goal) by pecking, the authors exposed them to a variety of detour tasks having lines as a barrier. A preview phase was introduced, during which the pigeons were not allowed to peck at the monitor. Results of a set of experiments suggest that our pigeons successfully learned to solve these tasks, that they came to take an efficient strategy as the barriers became complex, and that they possibly preplan its solution, at least on familiar, well-practiced tasks. 相似文献
95.
For 35 to 39 days, four observers wore continuously left-right reversing spectacles which pseudoscopically reverse the order of binocular disparity and direction of convergence. In three tests, we investigated how the visual system copes with the transformation of depth and distance information due to the reversing spectacles. In stereogram observation, after a few days of wearing the spectacles. the observers sometimes perceived a depth order which was opposite to the depth order that they had perceived in the pre-spectacle-wearing period. Monocular depth cues contributed more to depth perception in the spectacle-wearing period than they did in the pre-spectacle-wearing period. While the perceived distance significantly decreased during the spectacle-wearing period, we found no evidence of adaptive change in distance perception. The results indicate that the visual system adapts itself to the transformed situation by not only changing the processing of disparity but also by changing the relative efficiency of each cue in determining apparent depth. 相似文献
96.
Yoshiyuki Ueda Lei Chen Jonathon Kopecky Emily S. Cramer Ronald A. Rensink David E. Meyer Shinobu Kitayama Jun Saiki 《Cognitive Science》2018,42(1):286-310
While some studies suggest cultural differences in visual processing, others do not, possibly because the complexity of their tasks draws upon high‐level factors that could obscure such effects. To control for this, we examined cultural differences in visual search for geometric figures, a relatively simple task for which the underlying mechanisms are reasonably well known. We replicated earlier results showing that North Americans had a reliable search asymmetry for line length: Search for long among short lines was faster than vice versa. In contrast, Japanese participants showed no asymmetry. This difference did not appear to be affected by stimulus density. Other kinds of stimuli resulted in other patterns of asymmetry differences, suggesting that these are not due to factors such as analytic/holistic processing but are based instead on the target‐detection process. In particular, our results indicate that at least some cultural differences reflect different ways of processing early‐level features, possibly in response to environmental factors. 相似文献
97.
Kazuo Fujita 《Animal cognition》2009,12(4):575-585
Whereas evidence for metacognition by nonhuman primates has been obtained in great apes and old world monkeys, it is weaker
in new world monkeys. For instance, capuchin monkeys may fail to recognize their own knowledge of the location of invisible
bait. In the present study, we tested whether tufted capuchin monkeys would flexibly change their behavior in a delayed matching-to-sample
(DMTS) test depending upon the strength of their memory trace of the sample. In Experiment 1, two monkeys were tested on a
modified 9-alternative DMTS task with various delays on a computerized display. In some trials, the monkeys could choose whether
to go for a memory test or for a simple key touch as an escape from the test. In other trials, they were forced to go for
the memory test. Both monkeys escaped from the memory test more often when their matching accuracy on forced tests was lower.
In one of the monkeys, the matching accuracies on chosen memory tests decreased more slowly as a function of delay length,
and were higher after long delays than those on forced memory tests. This suggests that at least one capuchin monkey was able
to recognize the strength of his own memory trace. Experiment 2 employed occasional no-sample tests, in which the monkeys
faced the task choice without presentation of any sample for the trial. The monkey who was successful in Experiment 1 declined
the memory test more often in no-sample trials than regular trials, further indicating metamemory in this individual. In Experiment
3, this successful monkey received a task, in which he was sometimes able to choose between shape MTS or texture MTS tasks.
However, his matching accuracies did not differ between chosen tasks and forced tasks. Thus, the metamemory possessed by this
new world monkey species may be more like a flag, showing strength of memory trace, than an elaborate representation showing
details of the memory trace. 相似文献
98.
This study investigated the effect of exogenous spatial attention on auditory information processing. In Experiments 1, 2 and 3, temporal order judgment tasks were performed to examine the effect. In Experiment 1 and 2, a cue tone was presented to either the left or right ear, followed by sequential presentation of two target tones. The subjects
judged the order of presentation of the target tones. The results showed that subjects heard both tones simultaneously when
the target tone, which was presented on the same side as the cue tone, was presented after the target tone on the opposite
side. This indicates that spatial exogenous attention was aroused by the cue tone, and facilitated subsequent auditory information
processing. Experiment 3 examined whether both cue position and frequency influence the resulting information processing. The same effect of spatial
attention was observed, but the effect of attention to a certain frequency was only partially observed. In Experiment 4, a tone fusion judgment task was performed to examine whether the effect of spatial attention occurred in the initial stages
of hearing. The result suggests that the effect occurred in the later stages of hearing. 相似文献
99.
This study proposes a new item parameter linking method for the common-item nonequivalent groups design in item response theory
(IRT). Previous studies assumed that examinees are randomly assigned to either test form. However, examinees can frequently
select their own test forms and tests often differ according to examinees’ abilities. In such cases, concurrent calibration
or multiple group IRT modeling without modeling test form selection behavior can yield severely biased results. We proposed
a model wherein test form selection behavior depends on test scores and used a Monte Carlo expectation maximization (MCEM)
algorithm. This method provided adequate estimates of testing parameters. 相似文献
100.
James R. Anderson Hika Kuroshima Kazuo Fujita 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2017,70(7):1254-1262
Young human children have been shown to learn less effectively from video or televised images than from real-life demonstrations. Although nonhuman primates respond to and can learn from video images, there is a lack of direct comparisons of task acquisition from video and live demonstrations. To address this gap in knowledge, we presented capuchin monkeys with video clips of a human demonstrator explicitly hiding food under one of two containers. The clips were presented at normal, faster than normal, or slower than normal speed, and then the monkeys were allowed to choose between the real containers. Even after 55 sessions and hundreds of video demonstration trials the monkeys’ performances indicated no mastery of the task, and there was no effect of video speed. When given live demonstrations of the hiding act, the monkeys’ performances were vastly improved. Upon subsequent return to video demonstrations, performances declined to pre-live-demonstration levels, but this time with evidence for an advantage of fast video demonstrations. Demonstration action speed may be one aspect of images that influence nonhuman primates’ ability to learn from video images, an ability that in monkeys, as in young children, appears limited compared to learning from live models. 相似文献