排序方式: 共有132条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
71.
Ryuhei Ueda Kuniaki Yanagisawa Hiroshi Ashida Nobuhito Abe 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2017,17(6):1210-1220
Do we actively maintain monogamous relationships by force of will, or does monogamy flow automatically? During functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), male participants in a romantic relationship performed the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to evaluate implicit attitudes toward adultery and a go/no-go task to measure prefrontal activity implicated in explicit executive control. Subsequently, they were engaged in a date-rating task in which they rated how much they wanted to date unfamiliar females. We found that the individuals with higher prefrontal activity during go/no-go task could regulate the interest for dates with unattractive females; moreover, the individuals with both a stronger negative attitude toward adultery and higher prefrontal activity could regulate their interest for dates with attractive females, and such individuals tended to maintain longer romantic relationships with a particular partner. These results indicate that regulation of amorous temptation via monogamous relationship is affected by the combination of automatic and reflective processes. 相似文献
72.
Adachi I Anderson JR Fujita K 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2011,125(1):84-90
Seven squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) previously trained on reverse-reward tasks were presented with the original "1-versus-4" task after a 5-year interval without reverse-reward experience (Experiment 1). None of them reliably selected the smaller food array; however, at around chance level, their performance was superior to when they were first exposed to the task almost 6 years previously, suggesting some long-term memory retention. One naive monkey consistently selected the larger array, as expected. In Experiment 2, trials consisting of 1 versus 1 piece of two qualitatively different types of food were interspersed among familiar 1-versus-4 trials. None of five monkeys tested reliably selected the less-preferred food to get the more preferred food as the reward, and one monkey scored below chance. However, when one piece of low-preference food was paired with four pieces of high-preference food (Experiment 3), all four monkeys tested avoided reaching for the latter and thereby obtained it as the reward; two monkeys obtained perfect scores on these trials. These two monkeys were trained on a specific qualitative reverse-reward pairing and then again tested on new pairings (Experiment 4), but transfer was incomplete. Compound trials that pit quantity against quality in novel ways appear taxing for squirrel monkeys, despite competence in reverse-reward on both dimensions separately. 相似文献
73.
A new laboratory procedure which allows the study of deceptive behavior in nonhuman primates is described. Pairs of tufted
capuchin monkeys faced each other in a food-competition contest. Two feeder boxes were placed between the monkeys. A piece
of food was placed in one of the boxes. The subordinate individual was able to see the food and to open the box to obtain
the bait. A dominant male was unable to see the food or to open the box but was able to take the food once the box was opened
by the subordinate. In experiment 1, two of four subordinate monkeys spontaneously started to open the unbaited box first
with increasing frequency. Experiment 2 confirmed that this "deceptive" act was not due to a drop in the rate of reinforcement
caused by the usurping dominant male, under the situation in which food sometimes automatically dropped from the opened box.
In experiment 3, two subordinate monkeys were rerun in the same situation as experiment 1. One of them showed some recovery
of the "deceptive" act but the other did not; instead the latter tended to position himself on the side where there was no
food before he started to open the box. Although the results do not clearly indicate spontaneous deception, we suggest that
operationally defined spontaneous deceptive behaviors in monkeys can be analyzed with experimental procedures such as those
used here.
Accepted after revision: 13 June 2001
Electronic Publication 相似文献
74.
Fujita K Kuroshima H Asai S 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2003,29(3):233-242
Four tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) were trained to choose from 2 hook-like tools, 1 of which successfully led to collecting food, whereas the other did not because of inappropriate spatial arrangement of the tool and the food. In Experiment 1, all of the monkeys successfully learned the basic task. The monkeys performed successfully with tools of novel colors and shapes in Experiments 2-5. These results demonstrate that the monkeys used the spatial arrangement of the tool and the food as a cue. However, they failed when there were obstacles (Experiment 6) or traps (Experiment 7) on the path along which the monkeys dragged tools. These results may suggest that capuchin monkeys understand the spatial relationship between 2 items, namely, food and the tool, but do not understand the spatial relationship among 3 items, namely, food, tool, and the environmental condition. The possible role of stimulus generalization is also considered. 相似文献
75.
Human infants perceive two rods moving in concert behind an occluder as one unitary rod. In four experiments we tested whether
pigeons also perceive unity of objects. Pigeons were trained on a matching-to-sample task to discriminate between one unitary
rod moving at a constant speed and two aligned rods moving together at the same speed. The latter stimulus was identical to
the former except for a gap in the center. In experiment 1, we tested pigeons in probe trials in which a rectangle occluded
the center of the sample rods, to see which comparison stimulus, the unitary rod or the aligned two rods, the subjects would
match to the sample. Two of the three subjects pecked at the two rods significantly more often than at the unitary rod. In
experiment 2, we trained the same pigeons to match the sample rods moving "in front of" the occluder. Pigeons persisted in
matching two separate rods to the unitary rod moving in front of the occluder. In experiments 3 and 4, we used a parallelogram
and an undulating shape as the occluder to alter the shape and the size of the portions above and below the occluder by the
motion of the sample rods. Both subjects chose the two rods significantly more often than chance in experiment 3 and one of
them did so in experiment 4. The results suggest that pigeons do not complete occluded portions even though the two elements
move in concert. These negative results suggest that some alternative way of identifying objects may have evolved in pigeons.
Accepted after revision: 2 May 2001
Electronic Publication 相似文献
76.
Despite their impressive cognitive abilities, avian species have shown less evidence for metacognition than mammals. We suspect that commonly used tasks such as matching to sample might be too demanding to allow metacognitive processing within birds’ working memory. Here, we examined whether pigeons could control their behavior as a function of knowledge levels on a three-item sequence learning task, a reference memory task supposedly requiring fewer working memory resources. The experiment used two types of lists differing in familiarity. One was familiar to the pigeons through repeated exposure, whereas the other was novel in every new session. In test sessions, pigeons could choose between a trial with a hint specifying the next item to peck and one with no hint. However, successful responses in trials with a hint resulted in lowered rates of primary reinforcement: .60 in the first test and .75 in the second. Results showed that two of four pigeons chose the trial with a hint significantly more often before receiving a novel list than the familiar list in the four sessions of the first test, and three did so in the second test. Impressively, one bird showed robust evidence in the very first sessions in both tests. These results suggest that pigeons may monitor their long-term knowledge states and thereby control their environment before starting to solve a task. 相似文献
77.
Morn K 《Behavior research methods》2007,39(3):574-578
The MORI technique reported in Mori (2003) has evolved into its second generation after the appearance of a new type of video projector with two advantages over the first generation. The new MORI technique uses a pair of DLP projectors instead of the LCD type. The improved technique does not require sideways framing of the movie for presentation as required formerly. Therefore, any ready-made movie clip can be used. Second, it utilizes a pair of circular polarizing filters instead of linear polarizers. Linear polarizing filters may leak the to-be-blocked light wave when they are tilted. Circular polarizing filters are less susceptible to that weakness. The effectiveness of the revised technique has been proved by several studies carried out in two separate laboratories in which undergraduates and preschoolers participated as eyewitnesses to observe simulated criminal events presented using this technique. 相似文献
78.
We investigated perceived range, perceived velocity, and perceived duration of the body rotating in the frontal plane (in roll). Specifically, to examine how shear to the otoliths in the inner ears and tactile pressure to the trunk affect judgments of range and velocity, in two experiments, we manipulated rotating range (30°–160°), rotating velocity (1.8°/sec to 9.6°/sec), mean tilt of the body (?60°, 0°, and 60°), and exposure to the visual vertical. Thirty-three normal or blindfolded participants made verbal judgments of range, velocity, and duration for each combination of these factors. The exponents of the power functions fitted to these judgments were, as a first approximation, .94, .61, and .84 for range, velocity, and duration, respectively, and perceived velocity was proportional to the ratio of perceived range to perceived duration (r = .91). These results suggest that the vestibular and somatosensory inputs are effective on judgments of range, but less so on judgments of velocity, and that perceived velocity may be determined as a ratio of perceived range to perceived duration. In addition, we found that (1) when the range the body has traveled is constant, the perceived range increases as the objective velocity decreases (proprioceptive τ effect); (2) self-motion through the tilted roll sometimes enlarges perceived range and perceived duration but reduces perceived velocity; and (3) the exposure to the visual vertical reduces variability of judgments for range and velocity and also reduces perceived range and perceived velocity of self-motion within a small range through the vertical roll. 相似文献
79.
Japanese 8-month-olds were tested to investigate the matching of particular lip movements to corresponding non-canonical sounds, namely a bilabial trill (BT) and a whistle (WL). The results showed that the infants succeeded in lip-voice matching for the bilabial trill, whereas they failed to do so for the whistle. 相似文献
80.
The decay of the peak intensity of the electron spin resonance signal associated with light-induced dangling bonds in a-Si(H) has been measured at room temperature as a function of time during very long intervals such as 3400 days after the intense pulsed illumination was turned off. The decay curve is fitted by an exponential function with a decay constant of 393 days and reaches a steady-state value smaller than the value taken before illumination. Such a long-term metastability of light-induced dangling bonds in a-Si:H is discussed in terms of reconstruction of the amorphous network occurring through hydrogen motion. 相似文献