全文获取类型
收费全文 | 143篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
145篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 15篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 1篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 1篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有145条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Janice Kaye McLelland 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1978,3(4):253-263
An item-by-item analysis was made of the scores of 178 five- to eighteen-year-old stutterers on the 27-item Cooper Chronicity Prediction Checklist for School-Aged Stutterers and a stuttering frequency estimate form. Results were interpreted as suggesting that male stutterers generally possess a more complex stuttering pattern than do females and that the definition as to what constitutes stuttering continues to vary significantly between parents, clinicians, and individuals labeled as stutterers. 相似文献
92.
The present study used a chimeric stimuli task to assess the magnitude of the left-hemispace bias in children with congenital unilateral brain damage (n = 46) as compared to typically developing matched controls (n = 46). As would be expected, controls exhibited a significant left-hemispace bias. In the presence of left hemisphere (LH) damage, the left-hemispace preference was found to be present, but attenuated, whereas right hemisphere (RH) damage resulted in a less lateralized process. Examination of lesion severity revealed that large lesions in the RH were associated with a reversal of the typical left-hemispace bias, while small lesions resulted in a left bias approximating that of controls. In contrast, the left-hemispace preference in children with LH damage was similar across lesion size. We conclude that damage to either hemisphere early in brain development may alter hemispheric preference for processing of nonverbal stimuli, and that at least in the case of RH damage, alteration of the normal perceptual asymmetry may depend on the interaction between lesion side and severity. 相似文献
93.
Ito TA Willadsen-Jensen EC Kaye JT Park B 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(4):818-823
Three studies examined the implicit evaluative associations activated by racially-ambiguous Black-White faces. In the context of both Black and White faces, Study 1 revealed a graded pattern of bias against racially-ambiguous faces that was weaker than the bias to Black faces but stronger than that to White faces. Study 2 showed that significant bias was present when racially-ambiguous faces appeared in the context of only White faces, but not in the context of only Black faces. Study 3 demonstrated that context produces perceptual contrast effects on racial-prototypicality judgments. Racially-ambiguous faces were perceived as more prototypically Black in a White-only than mixed-race context, and less prototypically Black in a Black-only context. Conversely, they were seen as more prototypically White in a Black-only than mixed context, and less prototypically White in a White-only context. The studies suggest that both race-related featural properties within a face (i.e., racial ambiguity) and external contextual factors affect automatic evaluative associations. 相似文献
94.
Motor speed is an important indicator and predictor of both cognitive and physical function. One common assessment of motor
speed is the finger-tapping test (FTT), which is typically administered as part of a neurological or neuropsychological assessment.
However, the FTT suffers from several limitations, including infrequent in-person administration, the need for a trained assessor
and dedicated equipment, and potential short-term sensory–motor fatigue. In this article, we propose an alternative method
of measuring motor speed, with face validity to the FTT, that addresses these limitations by measuring the interkeystroke
intervals (IKI) of familiar and repeated login data collected in the home during a subject’s regular computer use. We show
significant correlations between the mean tapping speeds from the FTT and the median IKIs of the nondominant (r = .77) and dominant (r = .70) hands, respectively, in an elderly cohort of subjects living independently. Finally, we discuss how the proposed method
for measuring motor speed fits well into the framework of unobtrusive and continuous in-home assessment. 相似文献
95.
ABSTRACTAlthough commonly considered the core product offered by visitor attractions such as museums, zoos, and heritage sites, the visitor experience has proved a difficult construct to both define and measure. This article reviews the concept of visitor experience, drawing from literature in tourism and leisure research as well as museum and visitor studies, and identifies a number of issues on which the literature presents multiple perspectives. By clarifying these issues, this article takes a first step toward building a shared vocabulary to describe and measure visitor experiences. The article presents a conceptual scheme that describes relationships among key factors, and a multifaceted model of the visitor experience that offers a way of characterizing both its content and intensity. The article thus provides a basis for future research designed to capture this elusive phenomenon. 相似文献
96.
Dorota Weziak-Bialowolska;Richard G. Cowden;Kaye V. Cook;Piotr Bialowolski;Andrea Ortega Bechara;Zhuo Job Chen;Ni Made Taganing Kurniati;Christiany Suwartono;Eileen McNeely;Tyler J. VanderWeele; 《European journal of social psychology》2024,54(2):449-464
We examine whether an orientation to promote good serves as a potential buffer against distress and facilitator of well-being outcomes in three non-Western samples with a recent history of conflict (Indonesia, Colombia, Mexico) and across three different time frames (1, 4 and 12 months). Longitudinal surveys provided data for cross-lagged panel modelling. A series of sensitivity analyses provided evidence for the robustness of results. Persons with an orientation to promote good reported greater well-being 1 month and 4 months post-assessment and reduced distress 1 month and 4 months post-assessment. The effects, with larger effect sizes, were also found for reverse associations. We did not find evidence of associations assessed at the 12-month interval with the exception of a negative association between an orientation to promote good and subsequent life satisfaction and happiness. Orientation to promote good may be relevant for population's mental health and well-being over the short- and medium-term. 相似文献
97.
Sex Roles - Past research has indicated that women who work in male-dominated fields, such as politics, face discrimination due to a stereotypically perceived poor fit between their gender and... 相似文献
98.
Shane Darke Sharlene Kaye Johan Duflou Julia Lappin 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2019,49(1):328-337
99.
Elizabeth A. Bates Joseph J. McCann Linda K. Kaye Julie C. Taylor 《Qualitative research in psychology》2017,14(4):459-481
The use of photo elicitation is limited within the field of psychology despite its theoretical and practical potential. It offers significant benefits as a qualitative method that could present a new and interesting way of exploring previously understood topics within the discipline. Within our discussion, we present a step-by-step guide outlining the key practical stages and ethical assurances involved in photo elicitation research, using our ongoing research as an illustrative example. It is intended that this could be used as a model of good practice for developing research paradigms beyond those typically used within the psychology discipline. 相似文献
100.
One of the major challenges to linguistic theory is the solution of what has been termed the "projection problem". Simply put, linguistics must account for the fact that starting from a data base that is both unsystematic and relatively small, a human child is capable of constructing a grammar that mirrors, for all intents and purposes, the adult system. In this article we shall address ourselves to the question of the learnability of a postulated subsystem of phonological structure: the stress system. We shall describe a computer program which is designed to acquire this subpart of linguistic structure. Our approach follows the "principles and parameters" model of Chomsky (1981a, b). This model is particularly interesting from both a computational point of view and with respect to the development of learning theories. We encode the relevant aspects of universal grammar (UG)--those aspects of linguistic structure that are presumed innate and thus present in every linguistic system. The learning process consists of fixing a number of parameters which have been shown to underlie stress systems and which should, in principle, lead the learner to the postulation of the system from which the primary linguistic data (i.e., the input to the learner) is drawn. We go on to explore certain formal and substantive properties of this learning system. Questions such as cross-parameter dependencies, determinism, subsets, and incremental versus all-at-once learning are raised and discussed in the article. The issues raised by this study provide another perspective on the formal structure of stress systems and the learnability of parameter systems in general. 相似文献