首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   329篇
  免费   17篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   6篇
  2013年   29篇
  2012年   12篇
  2011年   14篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   15篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   12篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   2篇
  1965年   2篇
  1951年   1篇
排序方式: 共有346条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
In a serial learning task given to subjects whose ages ranged from twenty to seventy, a decline in performance showed itself as a loss of speed in the thirties, a loss of accuracy in the forties, and a marked loss of both speed and accuracy in the fifties and sixties. Experiments on recall and reorganization of material verified that older subjects were forgetting more quickly than younger, but that one of their main difficulties was an inability to substitute new reponses in an already mentally formed sequence.

Older subjects were not introducing new learning features but accentuating normal tendencies, such as the repetition of the same error at the same serial position. In so far as subjects learned their repeated errors, final learning involved unlearning, with the often observable phenomenon of the previously learned error serving as the cue for the more recently learned correct response. In terms of schematic concepts of mental functions, serial learning difficulty was not so much the formation of a general “scheme” but its subsequent amendment. Preformed habits influenced both the learning procedure and its results, particularly in the constraints and expectancies which the making of one serial response had upon another.  相似文献   
72.
Three experiments are described which have attempted to identify some of the difficulties in recognition after interpolated recall. In the first, complex picture material was used, and, though subjects were able to recognize the original after interpolated recall, they did in fact recognize fewer items. This posed the query, why should the recall of some items depress the recognition of other unrecalled items? The second experiment showed that in recognition the stronger (correct) memory has an inhibitory effect on a weaker (also correct) memory. The third experiment therefore examined the hypothesis that the juxtaposition of better and worse known items raises the threshold of recognition of the worse known, and confirmed that the threshold of recognition for an item varies according to its context. The theoretical significance of this finding is briefly indicated.  相似文献   
73.
74.
This study examined the relationship between a number of cognitive problem-solving and life event variables and depressive symptoms in a sample of non-referred grade school children. The results indicated that higher levels of depressive symptoms were associated with an external locus of control, increased levels of objective and subjective life stress, and lower performance levels on an impersonal problem-solving task. Level of depressive symptoms was also found to be inversely related to socioeconomic status as measured by father's occupation. Contrary to prediction, no consistent relationship was found between depression and interpersonal problem-solving ability.This research is based on the first author's master's thesis submitted under the supervision of the second author to the Department of Psychology, University of Missouri-Columbia.  相似文献   
75.
76.
77.
Innovation is widely linked to cognitive ability, brain size, and adaptation to novel conditions. However, successful innovation appears to be influenced by both cognitive factors, such as inhibitory control, and non-cognitive behavioral traits. We used a multi-access box (MAB) paradigm to measure repeated innovation, the number of unique innovations learned across trials, by 10 captive spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Spotted hyenas are highly innovative in captivity and also display striking variation in behavioral traits, making them good model organisms for examining the relationship between innovation and other behavioral traits. We measured persistence, motor diversity, motivation, activity, efficiency, inhibitory control, and neophobia demonstrated by hyenas while interacting with the MAB. We also independently assessed inhibitory control with a detour cylinder task. Most hyenas were able to solve the MAB at least once, but only four hyenas satisfied learning criteria for all four possible solutions. Interestingly, neither measure of inhibitory control predicted repeated innovation. Instead, repeated innovation was predicted by a proactive syndrome of behavioral traits that included high persistence, high motor diversity, high activity and low neophobia. Our results suggest that this proactive behavioral syndrome may be more important than inhibitory control for successful innovation with the MAB by members of this species.  相似文献   
78.
In this article, I explore two perspectives on development that are central to how I think and work as an analyst, one drawn from the work of Hans Loewald and one from Melanie Klein. Loewald turned the usual psychoanalytic way of thinking, rooted in the past, on its head when he theorized that development proceeds by internalization of the parent’s future vision of the child and, by corollary, the analyst’s future vision of the patient. Using a vignette from Klein’s work with 10-year-old Richard, I show how the analyst’s image of the patient’s potential can facilitate growth and development. Melanie Klein also introduced a radical reordering of traditional psychoanalytic theory when she theorized that the mind develops and is structured as positions, not as successive phases. For Klein, the mind is organized in groupings of anxieties, defenses, and object-relations that are in a continuous state of oscillation throughout life independent of chronological age. Through a clinical vignette, I illustrate how one understands a patient differently when development is seen as occuring in momentary shifts between different levels of the personality rather than as stages over time.  相似文献   
79.
Point-light displays of human gait provide information sufficient to recognize the gender of a walker and are taken as evidence of the exquisite tuning of the visual system to biological motion. The authors revisit this topic with the goals of quantifying human efficiency at gender recognition. To achieve this, the authors first derive an ideal observer for gender recognition on the basis of center of moment (J. E. Cutting, D. R. Proffitt, & L. T. Kozlowski, 1978) and, with the use of anthropometric data from various populations, show optimal recognition of approximately 79% correct. Next, they perform a meta-analysis of 21 experiments examining gender recognition, obtaining accuracies of 66% correct for a side view and 71% for other views. Finally, results of the meta-analysis and the ideal observer are combined to obtain estimates of human efficiency at gender recognition of 26% for the side view and 47% for other views.  相似文献   
80.
It was hypothesized that exposure to complementary representations of the poor as happier and more honest than the rich would lead to increased support for the status quo. In Study 1, exposure to "poor but happy" and "rich but miserable" stereotype exemplars led people to score higher on a general measure of system justification, compared with people who were exposed to noncomplementary exemplars. Study 2 replicated this effect with "poor but honest" and "rich but dishonest" complementary stereotypes. In Studies 3 and 4, exposure to noncomplementary stereotype exemplars implicitly activated justice concerns, as indicated by faster reaction times to justice-related than neutral words in a lexical decision task. Evidence also suggested that the Protestant work ethic may moderate the effects of stereotype exposure on explicit system justification (but not implicit activation).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号