全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7618篇 |
免费 | 139篇 |
专业分类
7757篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 59篇 |
2020年 | 120篇 |
2019年 | 132篇 |
2018年 | 160篇 |
2017年 | 184篇 |
2016年 | 181篇 |
2015年 | 165篇 |
2014年 | 188篇 |
2013年 | 901篇 |
2012年 | 295篇 |
2011年 | 302篇 |
2010年 | 189篇 |
2009年 | 217篇 |
2008年 | 300篇 |
2007年 | 294篇 |
2006年 | 267篇 |
2005年 | 270篇 |
2004年 | 257篇 |
2003年 | 240篇 |
2002年 | 237篇 |
2001年 | 116篇 |
2000年 | 104篇 |
1999年 | 122篇 |
1998年 | 126篇 |
1997年 | 86篇 |
1996年 | 86篇 |
1995年 | 97篇 |
1994年 | 81篇 |
1993年 | 71篇 |
1992年 | 74篇 |
1991年 | 75篇 |
1990年 | 61篇 |
1989年 | 66篇 |
1988年 | 61篇 |
1987年 | 55篇 |
1986年 | 56篇 |
1985年 | 82篇 |
1984年 | 73篇 |
1983年 | 70篇 |
1982年 | 61篇 |
1981年 | 67篇 |
1980年 | 64篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 71篇 |
1977年 | 59篇 |
1976年 | 63篇 |
1975年 | 61篇 |
1974年 | 56篇 |
1973年 | 50篇 |
1966年 | 43篇 |
排序方式: 共有7757条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
Rachel E. Kahn Paul J. Frick Farrah N. Golmaryami Monica A. Marsee 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2017,45(3):583-596
In a sample of detained male adolescents (n = 107; Mean age = 15.50; SD = 1.30), we tested whether anxiety moderated the association of CU traits with self-report and computerized measures of affective (emotional reactivity) and cognitive (affective facial recognition and Theory of Mind [ToM]) empathy. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that CU traits were negatively associated with self-reports of affective empathy and this association was not moderated by level of anxiety. Significant interactions revealed that CU traits were negatively associated with cognitive empathy (self-report) only at high levels of anxiety, whereas CU traits were positively associated with cognitive empathy on the ToM task only at low levels of anxiety. CU traits were also associated with greater fear recognition accuracy at low levels of anxiety. Implications for understanding and treating different variants of CU traits (i.e., primary and secondary) are discussed. 相似文献
192.
Paul H. Thibodeau Latoya Crow Stephen J. Flusberg 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2017,24(5):1375-1386
While many scholars have pointed to the role of metaphor in explanation, relatively little experimental research has examined whether and how metaphors are used and understood in everyday explanatory discourse. Across 3 experiments, we investigated the nature and function of metaphor in explanation by drawing on a real-world example where the terms guardian and warrior were used to metaphorically explain the role of police officers. We found, first, that the associations participants brought to mind for these concepts differed depending on whether they had previously answered questions about law enforcement (e.g., associations for warrior emphasized aggression and violence rather than strength and bravery when participants had previously answered questions about policing). Second, people were almost evenly split in their judgment of which metaphor was more appropriate to explain the role of law enforcement; this preference was highly predictive of beliefs related to policing and the criminal justice system. Third, and most important, using these metaphors to explain the job of policing causally influenced attitudes toward law enforcement in a metaphor-congruent manner (i.e., exposure to the guardian metaphor led to more positive attitudes), a finding that could not be accounted for by basic lexical priming. These studies complement existing work that has identified metaphor as a mechanism for representing abstract concepts, but also highlight the communicative and explanatory, rather than representational, functions of metaphor by showing that metaphors can encapsulate and convey an array of structured attitudes and beliefs. 相似文献
193.
194.
195.
Hannah L. Filmer Roxanne Wells-Peris Paul E. Dux 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2017,79(4):1070-1077
It was long thought that a key characteristic of object substitution masking (OSM) was the requirement for spatial attention to be dispersed for the mask to impact visual sensitivity. However, recent studies have provided evidence questioning whether spatial attention interacts with OSM magnitude, suggesting that the previous reports reflect the impact of performance being at ceiling for the low attention load conditions. Another technique that has been employed to modulate attention in OSM paradigms involves presenting the target stimulus foveally, but with another demanding task shown immediately prior, and thus taxing executive/temporal attention. Under such conditions, when the two tasks occur in close temporal proximity relatively to greater temporal separation, masking is increased. However this effect could also be influenced by performance being at ceiling in some conditions. Here, we manipulated executive attention for a foveated target using a dual-task paradigm. Critically, ceiling performance was avoided by thresholding the target stimulus prior to it being presented under OSM conditions. We found no evidence for an interaction between executive attention load and masking. Collectively, along with the previous findings, our results provide compelling evidence that OSM as a phenomenon occurs independently of attention. 相似文献
196.
197.
Edwin J. C. G. van den Oord Jan Rispens Paul P. Goudena Marjolijn Vermande 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》2000,21(6):619
To gain insight into children's peer relations and their developmental significance, it is important to know not only how many relations children have but also to consider the structure of these relations. To demonstrate this, we used a multiple-study design in which we applied social network analyses to sociometric ratings and nominations from 1,082 4- to 5-year-old children in 83 school classes. Results showed that isolation in the classroom was more strongly associated with the choices that were given than those that were received. Gender plus the negative behavioral characteristics, aggressiveness and rejection, were the most important cues for social clustering. The structure of positive choices reflected the formation of friendship networks, whereas negative choices tended to be given directly to individual “problem” children. Finally, structure indices tapped aspects of children's relations that were not reflected in the traditional two-dimensional classification system and provided additional explanatory power for predicting developmental outcomes. 相似文献
198.
Parallel analysis (PA; Horn, 1965) is a technique for determining the number of factors to retain in exploratory factor analysis that has been shown to be superior to more widely known methods (Zwick & Velicer, 1986). Despite its merits, PA is not widely used in the psychological literature, probably because the method is unfamiliar and because modern, Windows-compatible software to perform PA is unavailable. We provide a FORTRAN-IMSL program for PA that runs on a PC under Windows; it is interactive and designed to suit the range of problems encountered in most psychological research. Furthermore, we provide sample output from the PA program in the form of tabled values that can be used to verify the program operation; or, they can be used either directly or with interpolation to meet specific needs of the researcher. 相似文献
199.
Paul Ceruzzi 《Knowledge, Technology, and Policy》2000,13(2):86-102
Beginning around the time of the Sputnik crisis of 1957, a number of specialized, defense-related firms moved to the Washington,
D.C. area. Their primary function was vague: they often defined their work as “systems integration”—but that was not well-defined.
Journalists, not understanding the nature of this activity, called the firms “Beltway Bandits,” a misnomer since nearly all
were located not around the Washington Beltway but rather close by one another in an area known as Tysons Corner, Virginia.
Though little understood and by 1997 all but gone as independent companies, these firms laid the foundation for one of the
most vibrant and economically successful regions of the country: Tysons Corner.
Tysons is now an anchor for Internet activity and probably has more high-technology software activity than anyplace, save
Silicon Valley, in the United States. It is also home to one of the most successful retail centers in the northeastern United
States. Tysons is a classic “Edge City” in the words of urban geographer Joel Garreau: tremendous commercial vitality yet
none of the qualities one associates with a “community.” Tysons Corner has no post office or ZIP code of its own, no political
boundary, no subway or railroad station, no library, public school, park, or town square. Yet is is an easily-recognized “place.”
This study looks at the area, its origins, and its present status, with a view toward some general comments about the place
for such “Edge Cities” in the country’s future. 相似文献
200.