全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1011篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
1020篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 26篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 43篇 |
2018年 | 45篇 |
2017年 | 41篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 96篇 |
2012年 | 54篇 |
2011年 | 58篇 |
2010年 | 44篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 32篇 |
2007年 | 49篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1020条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
To investigate the contested role of notional number in English subject-verb agreement, we used a sentence completion task
to examine agreement with minimally different subject noun-phrases, such as the gang on the motorcycles and the gang near
the motorcycles. These contrasting phrases biased different notional construals of collective nouns, such as gang, which are
normally ambiguous between plural (distributed) and singular (collected) construals. With subjects biased toward spatial distribution,
such as gang on motorcycles, more plural verbs occurred in speakers’ sentence completions than in sentence completions with
a bias toward spatial collection such as gang near motorcycles. This offers strong evidence regarding both the existence and
the magnitude of notional effects on subject-verb number agreement in English. 相似文献
44.
45.
46.
47.
Weil J Ormond K Peters J Peters K Biesecker BB LeRoy B 《Journal of genetic counseling》2006,15(2):85-93
Nondirectiveness has been a guiding principle for genetic counseling since the founding of the profession. However, its efficacy and appropriateness in this role have been frequently questioned. A workshop at the 2003 Annual Education Conference of the National Society of Genetic Counselors provided audience participation in a discussion of these issues. Participants presented arguments for and against nondirectiveness as a central ethos. They described complex personal transitions in adapting what they had learned about nondirectiveness during training to the realities of the workplace. There was support for flexible approaches to genetic counseling, with varying adherence to nondirectiveness, based on client and family needs and values, clinical circumstances, and desired counseling outcomes. The discussion supports the use of clinical experience, outcomes research, and the experience of other professions to move beyond nondirectiveness and more accurately identify the theoretical bases that underlie genetic counseling in the variety of circumstances in which it is currently practiced. 相似文献
48.
49.
A ladle was recalled as being taller by participants who observed tedious removal of sand from it with a small teaspoon than by those who observed removal with a larger spoon. A second experiment showed that the number of darts thrown in order to hit a target correlated negatively with memory estimates of the size of the target, a finding replicated in a third experiment with size estimates made while the target was visible. The first two experiments suggest that the way an object is used can influence memory of its size. The third experiment supports the hypothesis that in vivo size estimation of familiar objects may employ a mechanism that derives size from memory and that size memory can be distorted by the way an object was used. 相似文献
50.
Previous work on human memory has shown that prompting participants with false events and self-relevant information via different types of media such as narratives, edited 2-dimensional images, and mental imagery creates false memories. This study tested a new form of media for studying false memory formation: Immersive Virtual Environment Technology (IVET). Using this tool, we examined how memory was affected by viewing dynamic simulations of avatars performing novel actions. In the study, 55 preschool and elementary children were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 memory prompt conditions (idle, mental imagery, IVET simulation of another child, or IVET simulation of self). Each child was questioned 3 different times: once before the memory prompt, once immediately after the memory prompt, and once approximately 5 days after the memory prompt. Results showed that preschool children were equally likely to develop false memories regardless of memory prompt condition. But, for elementary children, the mental imagery and IVET self conditions caused significantly more false memories than the idle condition. Implications regarding the use of digital media in courtroom settings, clinical therapy settings, entertainment, and other applications are discussed. 相似文献