全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2240篇 |
免费 | 87篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2328篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 54篇 |
2017年 | 82篇 |
2016年 | 77篇 |
2015年 | 57篇 |
2014年 | 62篇 |
2013年 | 256篇 |
2012年 | 105篇 |
2011年 | 108篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 84篇 |
2007年 | 98篇 |
2006年 | 80篇 |
2005年 | 66篇 |
2004年 | 80篇 |
2003年 | 63篇 |
2002年 | 63篇 |
2001年 | 38篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 28篇 |
1992年 | 30篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 32篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 21篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1966年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2328条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
The authors propose 2 categories of situational moderators of gender in negotiation: situational ambiguity and gender triggers. Reducing the degree of situational ambiguity constrains the influence of gender on negotiation. Gender triggers prompt divergent behavioral responses as a function of gender. Field and lab studies (1 and 2) demonstrated that decreased ambiguity in the economic structure of a negotiation (structural ambiguity) reduces gender effects on negotiation performance. Study 3 showed that representation role (negotiating for self or other) functions as a gender trigger by producing a greater effect on female than male negotiation performance. Study 4 showed that decreased structural ambiguity constrains gender effects of representation role, suggesting that situational ambiguity and gender triggers work in interaction to moderate gender effects on negotiation performance. 相似文献
102.
Dual scaling is a set of related techniques for the analysis of a wide assortment of categorical data types including contingency tables and multiple-choice, rank order, and paired comparison data. When applied to a contingency table, dual scaling also goes by the name "correspondence analysis," and when applied to multiple-choice data in which there are more than 2 items, "optimal scaling" and "multiple correspondence analysis. " Our aim of this article was to explain in nontechnical terms what dual scaling offers to an analysis of contingency table and multiple-choice data. 相似文献
103.
Grammatical agreement flags the parts of sentences that belong together regardless of whether the parts appear together. In English, the major agreement controller is the sentence subject, the major agreement targets are verbs and pronouns, and the major agreement category is number. The authors expand an account of number agreement whose tenets are that pronouns acquire number lexically, whereas verbs acquire it syntactically but with similar contributions from number meaning and from the number morphology of agreement controllers. These tenets were instantiated in a model using existing verb agreement data. The model was then fit to a new, more extensive set of verb data and tested with a parallel set of pronoun data. The theory was supported by the model's outcomes. The results have implications for the integration of words and structures, for the workings of agreement categories, and for the nature of the transition from thought to language. 相似文献
104.
Keetch KM Schmidt RA Lee TD Young DE 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2005,31(5):970-978
Differing viewpoints concerning the specificity and generality of motor skill representations in memory were compared by contrasting versions of a skill having either extensive or minimal specific practice. In Experiments 1 and 2, skilled basketball players more accurately performed set shots at the foul line than would be predicted on the basis of the performance at the nearby locations, suggesting considerable specificity at this distance. This effect was replicated even when the lines on the court were obscured (in Experiment 2). However, the effect was absent when jump shots were executed in Experiment 3. The authors argue that massive levels of practice at 1 particular member of a class of actions produce specific effects that allow this skill to stand out from the other members of the class, giving it the status of an especial skill. Various theoretical views are proposed to account for the development of these skills. 相似文献
105.
Kathleen?M.?RospendaEmail author Judith?A.?Richman Jennifer?L.?Z.?Ehmke Kenneth?W.?Zlatoper 《Journal of business and psychology》2005,20(1):95-110
We examined cross-sectional and lagged effects of sexual harassment (SH) and generalized workplace harassment (GWH) on incidence of self-reported illness, injury, or assault in a sample of over 1,500 university employees. SH and GWH, but not other job stressors, were related to increased odds of illness, injury, or assault. This was true when SH, GWH, and illness, injury, or assault were measured at the same time point, as well as when SH and GWH were measured in year prior to illness, injury, or assault.The data presented here are part of an ongoing longitudinal study of workplace harassment conducted at the Department of Psychiatry of the University of Illinois at Chicago. This research was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (grants AA09989 and AA13332). Portions of this paper were presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, April 2004, Chicago, Illinois. The data were collected by the Survey Research Laboratory at the University of Illinois at Chicago. 相似文献
106.
Torre DM Wang NY Meoni LA Young JH Klag MJ Ford DE 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2005,35(2):146-153
Physicians frequently are early adopters of healthy behaviors based on their knowledge and economic resources. The mortality patterns of physicians in the United States, particularly suicide, have not been rigorously described for over a decade. Previous studies have shown lower all-cause mortality among physicians yet reported conflicting results about cause-specific mortality such as suicide. In this study, we compared all-cause and cause-specific mortality in a sample of physicians to the age-gender matched general U.S. population from 1948 through 1998. We also compared the mortality experience across medical specialties. The risk of all-cause mortality was 56% lower than expected in men, and 26% lower in women, compared to the general population. Standardized mortality ratios (SMRs) were markedly lower for diseases strongly linked to smoking, e.g., cardiovascular diseases, respiratory diseases, and lung cancer. Suicide was the only cause of death where risk was greater than the general population. Overall, we found that physicians are at substantially lower risk of dying compared to the general population for all causes of death except suicide. The findings for suicide are strikingly different than other causes of death and should provide impetus for new research on the mental health of physicians. 相似文献
107.
Recent studies have shown that the presentation of concurrent linguistic context can lead to highly efficient performance
in a standard conjunction search task by the induction of an incremental search strategy (Spivey, Tyler, Eberhard, & Tanenhaus,
2001). However, these findings were obtained under anomalously slow speech rate conditions. Accordingly, in the present study,
the effects of concurrent linguistic context on visual search performance were compared when speech was recorded at both a
normal rate and a slow rate. The findings provided clear evidence that the visual search benefit afforded by concurrent linguistic
context was contingent on speech rate, with normal speech producing a smaller benefit. Overall, these findings have important
implications for understanding how linguistic and visual processes interact in real time and suggest a disparity in the temporal
resolution of speech comprehension and visual search processes. 相似文献
108.
Much research suggests that words comprising more than one morpheme are represented in a “decomposed” manner in the visual
word recognition system. In the research presented here, we investigate what information is used to segment a word into its
morphemic constituents and, in particular, whether semantic information plays a role in that segmentation. Participants made
visual lexical decisions to stem targets preceded by masked primes sharing (1) a semantically transparent morphological relationship
with the target (e.g.,cleaner-CLEAN), (2) an apparent morphological relationship but no semantic relationship with the target (e.g.,corner-CORN), and (3) a nonmorphological form relationship with the target (e.g.,brothel-BROTH). Results showed significant and equivalent masked priming effects in cases in which primes and targets appeared to
be morphologically related, and priming in these conditions could be distinguished from nonmorphological form priming. We
argue that these findings suggest a level of representation at which apparently complex words are decomposed on the basis
of their morpho-orthographic properties. Implications of these findings for computational models of reading are discussed. 相似文献
109.
The authors manipulated movement amplitude in a bimanual circle-tracing task performed by 11 participants. With pacing frequency fixed, the systematic increase and decrease of circle diameter within a trial induced phase transitions from the asymmetric (33% of trials) to the symmetric bimanual circle-tracing pattern; the transitions resulted from a loss of stability in the asymmetric pattern. Tracing frequency varied inversely with circle diameter so that end-effector variability was minimized in a set of self-paced trials in which the circle diameter in a trial was fixed. In the amplitude-scaling trials, end-effector variability varied directly with circle diameter, a consistent speed-accuracy tradeoff. The results support the conclusion that movement amplitude is a nonspecific control parameter. The findings are discussed with reference to several factors, e.g., tactile feedback, the recruitment and suppression of biomechanical degrees of freedom, and the role those factors may play in stabilizing bimanual coordination patterns 相似文献
110.
A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Ashton MC Lee K Perugini M Szarota P de Vries RE Di Blas L Boies K De Raad B 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2004,86(2):356-366
Standard psycholexical studies of personality structure have produced a similar 6-factor solution in 7 languages (Dutch, French, German, Hungarian, Italian, Korean, Polish). The authors report the content of these personality dimensions and interpret them as follows: (a) a variant of Extraversion, defined by sociability and liveliness (though not by bravery and toughness); (b) a variant of Agreeableness, defined by gentleness, patience, and agreeableness (but also including anger and ill temper at its negative pole); (c) Conscientiousness (emphasizing organization and discipline rather than moral conscience); (d) Emotionality (containing anxiety, vulnerability, sentimentality, lack of bravery, and lack of toughness, but not anger or ill temper); (e) Honesty-Humility; (f) Intellect/Imagination/Unconventionality. A potential reorganization of the Big Five factor structure is discussed. 相似文献