全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1297篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
1301篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 28篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 41篇 |
2013年 | 150篇 |
2012年 | 64篇 |
2011年 | 61篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 59篇 |
2006年 | 49篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 53篇 |
2003年 | 51篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 31篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1301条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Koss KJ George MR Bergman KN Cummings EM Davies PT Cicchetti D 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2011,109(3):336-352
Marital conflict is a distressing context in which children must regulate their emotion and behavior; however, the associations between the multidimensionality of conflict and children’s regulatory processes need to be examined. The current study examined differences in children’s (N = 207, mean age = 8.02 years) emotions (mad, sad, scared, and happy) and behavioral strategies to regulate conflict exposure during resolved, unresolved, escalating, and child-rearing marital conflict vignettes. Children’s cortisol levels were assessed in relation to child-rearing and resolved conflict vignettes. Anger and sadness were associated with escalating and child-rearing conflicts, fearfulness was related to escalating and unresolved conflicts, and happiness was associated with resolution. Anger was associated with children’s strategies to stop conflict, whereas sadness was associated with monitoring and avoidant strategies. Cortisol recovery moderated the link between fearfulness and behavioral regulation. These results highlight the importance of children’s emotions and regulatory processes in understanding the impact of marital conflict. 相似文献
102.
Marieke L. Fransen Bob M. Fennis Kathleen D. Vohs 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(1):202-207
The present research extends work on the ‘the value from fit’ principle by showing that regulatory fit effects on persuasion and behavioral compliance are stronger for people high, as opposed to low, in private self-focus. Previous research has shown that people high in private self-focus are strongly affected by external information. In the current work, we demonstrated that people high (vs. low) in private self-focus are more persuaded by information that matches (vs. mismatches) activated self-knowledge. 相似文献
103.
Emily Suskin Laura Hercher Kathleen Erskine Aaron Komal Bajaj 《Journal of genetic counseling》2016,25(5):1032-1043
Since its introduction four years ago, noninvasive prenatal screening for fetal aneuploidy (NIPS) has been widely adopted as a screening tool for women at a high risk for fetal aneuploidy. As use expands into the general population, questions arise concerning the integration of NIPS into preexisting screening paradigms. This study aims to examine the use of NIPS in current practice among prenatal counselors, predominantly in the United States, in order to inform strategies for the optimal use of both new and existing screening techniques. We electronically surveyed 208 members of the National Society of Genetic Counselors to ascertain how NIPS is currently being used. Genetic counselors were also queried as to the advantages and disadvantages of offering NIPS to all patients regardless of a priori risk. Results indicate substantial variation in practice regarding which patients are offered NIPS and how counselors have incorporated this technology into existing screening routines. The majority of participants report offering NIPS in conjunction with another method of screening for fetal aneuploidy, indicating that NIPS is being used as an addition rather than as a replacement. These screening methods primarily include nuchal translucency (NT) (45.1 %, n = 78) and first trimester serum screening, with or without an NT (19.7 %, n = 34). Furthermore, the majority report that they would be concerned about losing the clinical value of an NT in a complete transition to NIPS (85.4 %, n = 164). Counselors are evenly split on the merits of expanding the use of NIPS to the general population (con: 55.3 %, n = 105; pro: 44.7 %, n = 85). The lack of consensus suggests that updated practice guidelines might benefit counselors. In addition, respondents emphasized the need to better educate patients and providers about the risks, benefits, and limitations of the test. 相似文献
104.
On each of five study-test trials, young and old adults attempted to memorize the same list of 60 words (e.g., bed, rest, awake), which were blocked according to their convergence on four corresponding associates. Half of the participants in each age group were given an explicit warning about the DRM paradigm prior to encoding and were asked to attempt to avoid recalling any associated but nonpresented words (e.g., sleep). Lists were presented auditorily at either a fast (1,250 msec/word) or a slow (2,500 msec/word) rate. Without a warning, the probability of veridical recall across trials increased for both age groups; however, the probability of false recall across trials decreased only for young adults. When a warning about false recall was provided, young adults virtually eliminated false recall by the second trial. Even though old adults also used warnings to reduce false recall on Trial 1, they were still unable to decrease false memories across the remaining four study-test trials. Old adults also reduced false recall more with slow than with fast presentation rates. Taken together, these findings suggest that old adults have a breakdown in spontaneous, self-initiated source monitoring as reflected by little change in false recall across study-test trials but a preserved ability to use experimenter-provided warnings or slow presentation rates to reduce false memories. 相似文献
105.
Two experiments examined adult humans’ folk physics (i.e., their naturally occurring and spontaneous understanding of the
physical world) using variations of trap-table problems used to study chimpanzees’ folk physics. When presented with these
problems, people unnecessarily avoided retrieving a reward by pulling a rake on the side of a table with a trapping hole—even
though it was highly unlikely that the hole would trap the reward. However, when the distance between the reward and the trap
was sufficiently large and the distance that the reward had to travel to be retrieved was sufficiently short, people preferred
to retrieve a reward by pulling the rake on the side of the table with the trap. These results underscore that behavior during
tool-use tasks has many possible causes, only one of which might be a subject’s folk physics. 相似文献
106.
107.
108.
Kathleen Pitts Faircloth 《Learning and motivation》1974,5(1):16-23
A comparison between the reinforcement strength of self-administered and externally imposed brain stimulation was undertaken. Rats performed on a chain schedule in order to receive brain stimulation, either selfadministered or imposed, during the second component of the chain. Response rates during the first component of the chain schedule indicated that self-stimulation was more reinforcing than imposed stimulation. In addition, contrast, both positive and negative, seemed to be present. It was concluded that reinforcement strength may be enhanced when the subject controls the presentation of reinforcement. 相似文献
109.
Kenneth I. Maton Douglas M. Teti Kathleen M. Corns Catherine C. Vieira-Baker Jacqueline R. Lavine Karen R. Gouze Daniel P. Keating 《American journal of community psychology》1996,24(4):551-587
Levels and correlates of parental support, peer support, partner support, and/or spiritual support among African American
and Caucasian youth were examined in three contexts: adolescent pregnancy (Study 1), first year of college (Study 2), and
adolescence and young adulthood (ages 15–29; Study 3). Partially consistent with a cultural specificity perspective, in different
contexts different support sources were higher in level and/or more strongly related to adjustment for one ethnic group than
the other. Among pregnant adolescents, levels of spiritual support were higher for African Americans than Caucasians; additionally,
peer support was positively related to well-being only for African Americans whereas partner support was positively related
to well-being only for Caucasians. Among college freshmen, family support was more strongly related to institutional and goal
commitment for African Americans than Caucasians; conversely, peer support was more strongly related to institutional and
goal commitment among Caucasians. Among 15 to 29-year-olds, levels of parental support and spiritual support were higher among
African Americans than Caucasians; additionally, spiritual support was positively related to self-esteem for African Americans
but not for Caucasians. Implications and limitations of the research are discussed.
The third study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant RO1 MH40963. We thank Monica Greene, Shea Lyda,
Wendy Stevenson, and the many undergraduate students who contributed to the three research projects. We also acknowledge the
very thoughtful and helpful comments of the anonymous reviewers and the editor, Edison Trickett. 相似文献
110.
Previous researchers have manipulated forewarning by providing premessage information about the topic and position of the upcoming communication, or the communicator's persuasive intent. Subjects in the present experiment were force-warned either 10 min prior to the communication, or just before the message began of the speaker's topic and position, persuasive intent, or topic only. As hypothesized, forewarning of the communicator's persuasive intent inhibited persuasion regardless of the length of the delay period, but forewarning of the topic and position required a delay in order to confer resistance to subsequent persuasion, suggesting that although both manipulations have been called “forewarning they may lead to reduced persuasion through different mechanisms. Foreknowledge of the source's topic, but not his position also increased resistance to persuasion when followed by a delay period. The results were discussed in terms of both cognitive and motivational mechanisms that may underly the persuasion inhibiting effects of forewarning. 相似文献