全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1373篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
1377篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 28篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 38篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 42篇 |
2013年 | 161篇 |
2012年 | 67篇 |
2011年 | 65篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 55篇 |
2007年 | 63篇 |
2006年 | 52篇 |
2005年 | 48篇 |
2004年 | 57篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 45篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1377条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
We examined associations between self-reported attachment anxiety and avoidance and responses to the Rorschach test. Seventy-two, nonpatient Israeli adults participated in a 2-session study. In the first session, they completed a self-report scale tapping the dimensions of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. In the second session, they completed the Rorschach test. The Rorschach was administered and coded according to Exner's (2001) Comprehensive System scoring. We found that self-reports of attachment anxiety were associated with Rorschach scores thought to indicate difficulties in regulating and controlling emotions and self-perceptions of being relatively helpless and unworthy. Self-reports of attachment avoidance were associated with Rorschach scores thought to reflect lack of acknowledgment of need states and maintenance of a grandiose self. We discuss the findings in terms of implicit psychodynamic processes inherent in attachment-system functioning. 相似文献
52.
Grammatical agreement flags the parts of sentences that belong together regardless of whether the parts appear together. In English, the major agreement controller is the sentence subject, the major agreement targets are verbs and pronouns, and the major agreement category is number. The authors expand an account of number agreement whose tenets are that pronouns acquire number lexically, whereas verbs acquire it syntactically but with similar contributions from number meaning and from the number morphology of agreement controllers. These tenets were instantiated in a model using existing verb agreement data. The model was then fit to a new, more extensive set of verb data and tested with a parallel set of pronoun data. The theory was supported by the model's outcomes. The results have implications for the integration of words and structures, for the workings of agreement categories, and for the nature of the transition from thought to language. 相似文献
53.
Recent studies have shown that the presentation of concurrent linguistic context can lead to highly efficient performance
in a standard conjunction search task by the induction of an incremental search strategy (Spivey, Tyler, Eberhard, & Tanenhaus,
2001). However, these findings were obtained under anomalously slow speech rate conditions. Accordingly, in the present study,
the effects of concurrent linguistic context on visual search performance were compared when speech was recorded at both a
normal rate and a slow rate. The findings provided clear evidence that the visual search benefit afforded by concurrent linguistic
context was contingent on speech rate, with normal speech producing a smaller benefit. Overall, these findings have important
implications for understanding how linguistic and visual processes interact in real time and suggest a disparity in the temporal
resolution of speech comprehension and visual search processes. 相似文献
54.
Much research suggests that words comprising more than one morpheme are represented in a “decomposed” manner in the visual
word recognition system. In the research presented here, we investigate what information is used to segment a word into its
morphemic constituents and, in particular, whether semantic information plays a role in that segmentation. Participants made
visual lexical decisions to stem targets preceded by masked primes sharing (1) a semantically transparent morphological relationship
with the target (e.g.,cleaner-CLEAN), (2) an apparent morphological relationship but no semantic relationship with the target (e.g.,corner-CORN), and (3) a nonmorphological form relationship with the target (e.g.,brothel-BROTH). Results showed significant and equivalent masked priming effects in cases in which primes and targets appeared to
be morphologically related, and priming in these conditions could be distinguished from nonmorphological form priming. We
argue that these findings suggest a level of representation at which apparently complex words are decomposed on the basis
of their morpho-orthographic properties. Implications of these findings for computational models of reading are discussed. 相似文献
55.
A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Ashton MC Lee K Perugini M Szarota P de Vries RE Di Blas L Boies K De Raad B 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2004,86(2):356-366
Standard psycholexical studies of personality structure have produced a similar 6-factor solution in 7 languages (Dutch, French, German, Hungarian, Italian, Korean, Polish). The authors report the content of these personality dimensions and interpret them as follows: (a) a variant of Extraversion, defined by sociability and liveliness (though not by bravery and toughness); (b) a variant of Agreeableness, defined by gentleness, patience, and agreeableness (but also including anger and ill temper at its negative pole); (c) Conscientiousness (emphasizing organization and discipline rather than moral conscience); (d) Emotionality (containing anxiety, vulnerability, sentimentality, lack of bravery, and lack of toughness, but not anger or ill temper); (e) Honesty-Humility; (f) Intellect/Imagination/Unconventionality. A potential reorganization of the Big Five factor structure is discussed. 相似文献
56.
Predicting preschoolers' externalizing behaviors from toddler temperament,conflict, and maternal negativity 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Rarely have researchers elucidated early childhood precursors of externalizing behaviors for boys and girls from a normative sample. Toddlers (N = 104; 52 girls) were observed interacting with a same-sex peer and their mothers, and indices of conflict-aggression, emotion and behavior dysregulation, parenting, and child externalizing problems were obtained. Results indicated that boys initiated more conflictual-aggressive interactions as toddlers and had more externalizing difficulties 2 years later, yet girls' (not boys') conflict-aggressive initiations at age 2 were related to subsequent externalizing problems. When such initiations were controlled for, emotional-behavioral undercontrol at age 2 also independently predicted externalizing problems at age 4. Moreover, the relation between conflict-aggressive initiations at age 2 and externalizing problems at age 4 was strongest for dysregulated toddlers. Finally, the relation between age 2 conflict-aggressive initiations and age 4 externalizing problems was strongest for those toddlers who incurred high levels of maternal negativity. These findings illustrate temperament by parenting connections in the development of externalizing problems. 相似文献
57.
When PD, an alphanumeric-colour synaesthete, is shown black digits or letters, each grapheme elicits a highly specific colour, called a "photism" (e.g., a 2 induces green, a Z induces brown). Previous experiments showed that photisms interfere with video-colour naming when the photism colour and the video colour are incongruent. Here we used coloured ambiguous graphemes that could be interpreted as either digits or letters depending on context (e.g., an ambiguous grapheme was interpreted as a 2 if presented within a block of digit trials, but as a Z if presented within a block of letter trials). Ambiguous graphemes were presented in video colours that were either congruent or incongruent with PD's photisms. Crucially, what was a congruent trial in one context was an incongruent trial in the other context. PD's pattern of response times indicated that identical graphemes could induce differently coloured photisms depending on their interpretation. This suggests that the meaning of graphemes ultimately determines their synaesthetic colour. 相似文献
58.
Low-frequency irregular words are named more slowly and are more error prone than low-frequency regular words (the regularity
effect). Rastle and Coltheart (1999) reported that this irregularity cost is modulated by the serial position of the irregular
grapheme-phoneme correspondence, such that words with early irregularities exhibit a larger cost than words with late ones.
They argued that these data implicate rule-based serial processing, and they also reported a successful simulation with a
model that has a rule-based serial component—the DRC model of reading aloud (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler,
2001). However, Zorzi (2000) also simulated these data with a model that operates solely in parallel. Furthermore, Kwantes
and Mewhort (1999) simulated these data with a serial processing model that has no rules for converting orthography to phonology.
The human data reported by Rastle and Coltheart therefore neither require a serial processing account, nor successfully discriminate
among a number of computational models of reading aloud. New data are presented wherein an interaction between the effects
of regularity and serial position of irregularity is again reported for human readers. The DRC model simulated this interaction;
no other implemented computational model does so. The present results are thus consistent with rule-based serial processing
in reading aloud. 相似文献
59.
Cognitive behaviour therapy is effective for obsessive-compulsive disorder and for obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders such as trichotillomania. Unfortunately, many people with these disorders, especially those living in rural areas, have limited access to treatment. Telephone-administered cognitive behaviour therapy may help address this problem. In a recent study of telephone treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder, we found that such treatment was often effective (42% in remission at post-treatment, and 47% in remission at 12-week follow-up). This article presents 2 case reports of the same treatment, applied to obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders (trichotillomania and compulsive skin picking). Treatment was associated with symptom reduction for both participants, although one subsequently relapsed. Possible reasons for relapse are discussed. The findings encourage further studies to identify the characteristics of people most likely to benefit from telephone treatment for spectrum disorders. 相似文献
60.