全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1320篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
1324篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 28篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 41篇 |
2013年 | 151篇 |
2012年 | 63篇 |
2011年 | 62篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 58篇 |
2006年 | 49篇 |
2005年 | 42篇 |
2004年 | 54篇 |
2003年 | 53篇 |
2002年 | 41篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1324条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
891.
Kathleen Crowley-Long 《Psychology of women quarterly》1998,22(1):113-130
There is a pervasive but implicit emphasis on a liberal feminist political agenda within the field of the psychology of women. This perspective is reflected by and inadvertently reproduced in many introductory texts to the psychology of women, although the perspective is usually unacknowledged. I argue that authors need to make their political perspectives explicit and to find ways to include other feminist viewpoints. Greater inclusion of socialist and radical feminist theoretical perspectives will strengthen the discipline of feminist psychology and promote greater inclusion of marginalized groups. 相似文献
892.
Kathleen E. Grady 《Psychology of women quarterly》1981,5(4):628-635
Feminist criticisms of selected aspects of research methods in psychology are presented. Data relevant to sex bias in topic selection, subject selection and single-sex designs, operationalization of variables, testing for sex differences, and interpretation of results are reviewed. Suggestions for achieving more sex fair research methods are discussed in terms of a more meticulous application of scientific method and an awareness of the sometimes subtle ways in which a sexist culture influences the research process. 相似文献
893.
Graham L. Staines Kathleen J. Pottick Deborah A. Fudge 《Psychology of women quarterly》1985,9(3):419-424
Previous research has established that wives' employment has a negative effect on the mental health of husbands. This result is not yet well understood. Analyzing survey data from a national sample of workers, this study focuses on the work domain to investigate this result further. Findings reveal that wives' employment has negative effects on husbands' job and life satisfaction. These results suggest that the overall negative mental health effect may derive, in part, from the occupational domain. 相似文献
894.
Kathleen V. Hoover-Dempsey Jeanne M. Plas Barbara Strudler Wallston 《Psychology of women quarterly》1986,10(1):19-34
Crying among adult professional women is a phenomenon experienced far more often than it is studied or publicly discussed. Guided by Sherifs (1982) theoretical work on gender identity, socialization, reference groups, and power, the authors explore several factors central to the topic. Among them are: the origins of conflict between women's experience of emotional expression and public tears; gender-related differences in crying and responses to crying; the role of reference groups in conflicting attitudes toward crying; and power in the workplace as influencing the meaning of crying. The role of images and imagery during times of conflicted and heightened emotion is discussed. It is suggested that most imagery related to crying in adults derives from perceptions of crying in infancy and that most responses to adults who cry are linked to an understanding of appropriate responses to a crying infant or child. Lewin's construct of behavior as a person-environment transaction is employed as a means of exploring the images experienced by the crier and the observer when female crying occurs in a professional situation. New images to guide more productive approaches to emotional expression in the workplace are suggested. 相似文献
895.
Norair Khachatryan M.A. Kathleen M. Heide Ph.D. Jordyn Rad B.A. Erich V. Hummel 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2016,34(6):709-725
Killings by juvenile homicide offenders (JHOs) who use accomplices have been increasing since the 1980s and currently represent approximately half of juvenile arrests for murder in the United States. Nevertheless, prior research has not compared JHOs who kill alone with JHOs who kill in groups. The present research followed up 30 years later on a sample of 59 male murderers and attempted murderers sentenced to adult prison. This study was designed to analyze whether lone and group JHOs differed on pre‐incarceration, incarceration, and post‐incarceration variables. Significant findings indicated that compared with lone offenders, group JHOs had a higher mean of pre‐homicide arrests and were more likely to be Black, have a pre‐homicide delinquent record, commit a crime‐related homicide offense, and target a stranger. With respect to post‐homicide variables, group JHOs were more likely to be released from prison and more likely to be rearrested. The two types of JHOs did not differ significantly in relation to the number of post‐release violent offenses. Preliminary implications of the findings and avenues for future investigation are discussed. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
896.
Kathleen M. Arnold David B. Daniel Jamie L. Jensen Mark A. McDaniel Elizabeth J. Marsh 《Applied cognitive psychology》2016,30(3):454-459
Knowing what skills underlie college success can allow students, teachers, and universities to identify and to help at‐risk students. One skill that may underlie success across a variety of subject areas is structure building, the ability to create mental representations of narratives (Gernsbacher, Varner, & Faust, 1990 ). We tested if individual differences in structure‐building ability predicted success in two college classes: introductory to psychology and introductory biology. In both cases, structure building predicted success. This effect was robust, with structure building explaining variance in course grades even after accounting for high school GPA and SAT scores (in the psychology course) or a measure of domain knowledge (in the biology course). The results support the claim that structure building is an important individual difference, one that is associated with learning in different domains. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
897.
The purpose of this study was to see how people perceive their own learning during a category learning task, and whether their perceptions matched their performance. In two experiments, participants were asked to learn natural categories, of both high and low variability, and make category learning judgements (CLJs). Variability was manipulated by varying the number of exemplars and the number of times each exemplar was presented within each category. Experiment 1 showed that participants were generally overconfident in their knowledge of low variability families, suggesting that they considered repetition to be more useful for learning than it actually was. Also, a correct trial, for a particular category, was more likely to occur if the previous trial was correct. CLJs had the largest increase when a trial was correct following an incorrect trial and the largest decrease when an incorrect trial followed a correct trial. Experiment 2 replicated these results, but also demonstrated that global CLJ ratings showed the same bias towards repetition. These results indicate that we generally identify success as being the biggest determinant of learning, but do not always recognise cues, such as variability, that enhance learning. 相似文献
898.
899.
Adults who move together to a shared musical beat synchronously as opposed to asynchronously are subsequently more likely to display prosocial behaviors toward each other. The development of musical behaviors during infancy has been described previously, but the social implications of such behaviors in infancy have been little studied. In Experiment 1, each of 48 14‐month‐old infants was held by an assistant and gently bounced to music while facing the experimenter, who bounced either in‐synchrony or out‐of‐synchrony with the way the infant was bounced. The infants were then placed in a situation in which they had the opportunity to help the experimenter by handing objects to her that she had ‘accidently’ dropped. We found that 14‐month‐old infants were more likely to engage in altruistic behavior and help the experimenter after having been bounced to music in synchrony with her, compared to infants who were bounced to music asynchronously with her. The results of Experiment 2, using anti‐phase bouncing, suggest that this is due to the contingency of the synchronous movements as opposed to movement symmetry. These findings support the hypothesis that interpersonal motor synchrony might be one key component of musical engagement that encourages social bonds among group members, and suggest that this motor synchrony to music may promote the very early development of altruistic behavior. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IaqWehfDm7c&feature=youtu.be 相似文献
900.
Wilhelm Hofmann Maike Luhmann Rachel R Fisher Kathleen D. Vohs Roy F. Baumeister 《Journal of personality》2014,82(4):265-277
Does trait self‐control (TSC) predict affective well‐being and life satisfaction—positively, negatively, or not? We conducted three studies (Study 1: N = 414, 64% female, Mage = 35.0 years; Study 2: N = 208, 66% female, Mage = 25.24 years; Study 3: N = 234, 61% female, Mage = 34.53 years). The key predictor was TSC, with affective well‐being and life satisfaction ratings as key outcomes. Potential explanatory constructs including goal conflict, goal balancing, and emotional distress also were investigated. TSC is positively related to affective well‐being and life satisfaction, and managing goal conflict is a key as to why. All studies, moreover, showed that the effect of TSC on life satisfaction is at least partially mediated by affect. Study 1's correlational study established the effect. Study 2's experience sampling approach demonstrated that compared to those low in TSC, those high in TSC experience higher levels of momentary affect even as they experience desire, an effect partially mediated through experiencing lower conflict and emotional distress. Study 3 found evidence for the proposed mechanism—that TSC may boost well‐being by helping people avoid frequent conflict and balance vice‐virtue conflicts by favoring virtues. Self‐control positively contributes to happiness through avoiding and dealing with motivational conflict. 相似文献