全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2099篇 |
免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
2137篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 26篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 67篇 |
2018年 | 94篇 |
2017年 | 83篇 |
2016年 | 95篇 |
2015年 | 58篇 |
2014年 | 72篇 |
2013年 | 238篇 |
2012年 | 120篇 |
2011年 | 140篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 70篇 |
2008年 | 106篇 |
2007年 | 86篇 |
2006年 | 81篇 |
2005年 | 59篇 |
2004年 | 52篇 |
2003年 | 71篇 |
2002年 | 51篇 |
2001年 | 38篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有2137条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
Katherine Wainwright Paul Romanowich Natashia Bibriescas 《International journal of psychology》2020,55(6):951-958
Perfectionism and impulsivity are multidimensional constructs. While different perfectionism dimensions are exclusively measured through self-reports, different impulsivity dimensions can be measured through self-report or behaviour via preferences for different rewards. This study explored differential associations between perfectionism and impulsivity based on both dimension and measurement modality (self-report/behavioural). We then examined whether adaptive or maladaptive perfectionism would be differentially associated with impulsivity. Two-hundred and six students completed two perfectionism and three impulsivity measures (two self-report; one behavioural). Two self-report impulsivity measures were associated with specific perfectionism dimensions, whereas the behavioural measure was not associated with perfectionism. Maladaptive perfectionism was associated with decreasing impulsivity, whereas adaptive perfectionism was associated with increasing impulsivity. Perfectionism related to impulsivity differently depending on how each construct was measured. 相似文献
56.
57.
Katherine E. Gentry Rebecca N. Lewis Hunter Glanz Pedro I. Simes rpd S. Nyri Michael S. Reichert 《Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science》2020,11(5)
The multifaceted ability to produce, transmit, receive, and respond to acoustic signals is widespread in animals and forms the basis of the interdisciplinary science of bioacoustics. Bioacoustics research methods, including sound recording and playback experiments, are applicable in cognitive research that centers around the processing of information from the acoustic environment. We provide an overview of bioacoustics techniques in the context of cognitive studies and make the case for the importance of bioacoustics in the study of cognition by outlining some of the major cognitive processes in which acoustic signals are involved. We also describe key considerations associated with the recording of sound and its use in cognitive applications. Based on these considerations, we provide a set of recommendations for best practices in the recording and use of acoustic signals in cognitive studies. Our aim is to demonstrate that acoustic recordings and stimuli are valuable tools for cognitive researchers when used appropriately. In doing so, we hope to stimulate opportunities for innovative cognitive research that incorporates robust recording protocols. This article is categorized under:
- Neuroscience > Cognition
- Psychology > Theory and Methods
- Neuroscience > Behavior
- Neuroscience > Cognition
58.
Psychometric properties of the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey 1991) within an eating disordered sample seeking treatment (N = 238) and differences among eating disorder diagnostic groups on the PAI were examined. The PAI showed acceptable alpha coefficients, item-total correlations, and interitem correlations. The factor structure was similar to that reported by Morey (1991), with the addition of another factor related to interpersonal coolness and distance. Those with binge eating disorder (BED) reported fewer problems and less distress in general compared to other eating disordered groups. The BED and bulimia nervosa groups were different from the anorexia nervosa groups in frequency of matching on two PAI clusters. Use of the PAI with an eating disordered population and its utility in understanding eating disorder diagnostic groups is supported. 相似文献
59.
In two experiments, we investigated whether reading background information benefits memory for text content by influencing the amount of content encoded or the organization of the encoded content. In Experiment 1, half of the participants read background information about the issues to be discussed in the text material, whereas half did not. All the participants were then tested for free recall and cued recall of text content. Free recall was greater for individuals who read issue information than for those who did not. The groups did not differ on cued recall, suggesting that background information did not facilitate the encoding of more text content. Measures of representational organization indicated that increased recall in the issue information group resulted from better organization of content in memory. Experiment 2 extended these findings, using background information about text sources, demonstrated that the efficacy of background information depends on the semantic relationship between that information and text content. 相似文献
60.
Research indicates that guided imagery experiences can be mistaken for actual experiences under some circumstances. One explanation for such effects is that memory representations of guided imagery and actual events contain similar phenomenal characteristics such as sensory and contextual details, making the source of the events less distinguishable. This study examined this prediction, comparing memory characteristic ratings for guided imagery experiences with those for memories of perceived and natural imagery events (e.g., fantasies). Results replicated previous findings for the difference between perceived and natural imagery memories. Guided imagery ratings were also lower than those for perceived memories for most sensory details (sound, smell, and taste) and temporal details. However, guided imagery ratings for reflective details were lower than both perceived and natural imagery memory ratings. Thus, guided imagery was similar to natural imagery with respect to sensory details, but similar to perceived memories with respect to reflective details. 相似文献