全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1497篇 |
免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
1518篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 23篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 43篇 |
2019年 | 53篇 |
2018年 | 79篇 |
2017年 | 73篇 |
2016年 | 68篇 |
2015年 | 41篇 |
2014年 | 62篇 |
2013年 | 184篇 |
2012年 | 97篇 |
2011年 | 98篇 |
2010年 | 60篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 81篇 |
2007年 | 57篇 |
2006年 | 60篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 42篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 34篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1518条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
331.
A wealth of prior research has shown that testing can improve subsequent learning of the initially tested material. In contrast,
only one recent study has shown that an interim test over prior material can improve learning of subsequent new material (i.e.,
an interim-test effect). Five experiments replicated and extended this initial work by exploring the extent to which interim test effects generalize
to complex text material. Participants were prompted to recall each section of an expository text before moving on to study
the next section, or were only prompted to recall after the final section. In all experiments, recall of the final, target
section was greater when prior sections had received interim tests versus no interim tests. Experiment 3 established that the effect was due to interim testing in particular rather than to intervening activity in general. Experiment 4 established that the effect was not due to test expectancy differences. In contrast to prior research, Experiment 4 also provided evidence that the effect is not due to release from proactive interference. We discuss other possible mechanisms
underlying interim-test effects with text, including shifting to more effective encoding strategies. 相似文献
332.
333.
Piderman KM Lapid MI Stevens SR Ryan SM Somers KJ Kronberg MT Clark MM Rummans TA 《The journal of pastoral care & counseling : JPCC》2011,65(1-2):3:1-311
This study's aims were to describe the spirituality of depressed elderly psychiatric inpatients and to examine associations among spirituality, depression, and quality of life (QOL). Forty-five persons participated. Most reported frequent, stable spiritual practices and experiencing spiritual comfort and guidance. Some reported spiritual distress and changes in spirituality. During hospitalization, participants demonstrated increased spiritual well-being (SWB) and peacefulness, and reduced hopelessness, worthlessness, and guilt. Positive associations were found between SWB and QOL and negative associations between SWB and depression. 相似文献
334.
Catriona M. Morrison Katherine W. Hirsh Tameron Chappell Andrew W. Ellis 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(4):435-459
An important determinant of picture and word naming speed is the age at which the names were learned (age of acquisition). Two related interpretations of these effects are that they reflect differences between words in their cumulative frequency of use, or that they reflect differences in the amount of time early and lateacquired words have spent in lexical memory. Both theories predict that differences between early and late-acquired words will be less apparent in older than younger adults. Two experiments are reported in which younger and older adults read words varying in age of acquisition or frequency, or named objects varying in age of acquisition. There was an observed effect of word frequency only for young adults' word naming. In contrast, strong age of acquisition effects were found for both the young and the old participants. The implications of these results for theories of how age of acquisition might affect lexical processing are discussed. 相似文献
335.
Michelle M. Martel Molly Nikolas Katherine Jernigan Karen Friderici Joel T. Nigg 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2012,40(8):1223-1236
Oppositional-Defiant Disorder (ODD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) are highly comorbid, a phenomenon thought to be due to shared etiological factors and mechanisms. Little work has attempted to chart multiple-level-of-analysis pathways (i.e., simultaneously including biological, environmental, and trait influences) to ODD and ADHD, the goal of the present investigation. 559 children/adolescents (325 boys) between the ages of 6 and 18 participated in a multi-stage, comprehensive diagnostic procedure. 148 were classified as ODD; 309 were classified as ADHD, based on parent, teacher, and clinician ratings. Children provided buccal or salivary samples of DNA, assayed for select markers in DRD4 and 5HTT. Parents completed the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire and the California Q-Sort. Children completed the Child Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Correlational associations consistent with multiple-level-of-analysis pathways to ODD and ADHD emerged. For ODD, children with the short allele of the 5HTT promoter polymorphism had higher neuroticism and ODD symptoms regardless of level of self-blame in relation to inter-parental conflict, whereas children without this allele had more ODD symptoms only in the context of more self-blame for inter-parental conflict. For ADHD (and ODD), children homozygous for the long allele of DRD4 120?bp insertion polymorphism had lower conscientiousness when exposed to inconsistent parenting, whereas children without this genotype were more resilient to effects of inconsistent discipline on conscientiousness. Thus, ODD and ADHD appear to demonstrate somewhat distinct correlational associations between etiological factors and mechanisms consistent with pathway models using a multiple-level-of-analysis approach. 相似文献
336.
Katherine Nelson 《New Ideas in Psychology》2009,27(2):275-287
Wittgenstein's positions on word learning, rules of use, and the impossibility of a private language, as expounded in his Philosophical Investigations, are examined in relation to issues of early child word learning. Current theoretical positions in the cognitivist mode are contrasted with the social cultural pragmatic approach, and each is compared to the principles that Wittgenstein advanced. Bloom's [(2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press] version of the cognitivist theory rejects most of the principles that Wittgenstein advanced, relying on innate cognitive endowments to explain children's success in word learning, using the word-referent mapping paradigm. Nelson's “use without meaning” and Tomasello's [(2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press] social-pragmatic model of learning from use are presented as representative of Wittgensteinian principles that meaning exists in and is inferred from the uses of words within communal activities (“language games” in “forms of life”). 相似文献
337.
Social Psychology of Education - In Chile, as in other countries, there are large gender gaps in reading achievement. One factor that may explain some of these results is male and female... 相似文献
338.
Andres De Los Reyes Amelia Aldao Sarah A. Thomas Samantha Daruwala Anna J. Swan Michael Van Wie Katherine Goepel William V. Lechner 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2012,34(3):308-322
We examined self-reported and parent-reported adolescent social anxiety symptoms and objective baseline measures of psychophysiological flexibility in 62 families. Measures completed by 31 adolescents referred for a clinical screening evaluation for social anxiety were compared to an age- and gender-matched community control sample of 31 adolescents (total sample: age range 14 to 17?years; 22 boys and 40 girls; M?=?15.32?years; SD?=?1.1). Clinic referred adolescents reported significantly fewer social anxiety symptoms than parents reported about adolescents. Further, for all adolescents, self-reported social anxiety symptoms exhibited low correspondence with objective psychophysiological measures. Yet, both measures uniquely discriminated between adolescents on whether they were clinic referred for a social anxiety screening. Further, adolescent self-reported social anxiety symptoms exhibited high levels of internal consistency and convergent validity. Findings indicate that researchers and practitioners should refrain from using disagreements between adolescent self-reports and other measures (e.g., parent report, objective measures) as indicators of the veracity of adolescent self-reports. 相似文献
339.
Violence has been identified as a significant risk behavior among children and adolescents. Dangerousness is a factor in psychiatric hospitalizations and out-of-community placements. However, there is only a small amount of research that has investigated the mental health co-morbidities and treatment outcomes of violent children and adolescents. A random sample of children and adolescents in residential placements through the State of Florida's child welfare system were studied. Cases were classified into three levels of dangerousness. Results indicated that dangerousness was associated with much elevated mental health co-morbidity. Also, cases who were dangerous at the time of admission had less developed peer and moral/spiritual strengths than did other cases. Although dangerous cases had worse dispositional outcomes, there was strong evidence that these cases derived the most clinical benefit from residential treatment. 相似文献
340.