全文获取类型
收费全文 | 57645篇 |
免费 | 2457篇 |
国内免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
60124篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 715篇 |
2019年 | 849篇 |
2018年 | 1214篇 |
2017年 | 1217篇 |
2016年 | 1278篇 |
2015年 | 902篇 |
2014年 | 1089篇 |
2013年 | 5073篇 |
2012年 | 2028篇 |
2011年 | 2033篇 |
2010年 | 1214篇 |
2009年 | 1241篇 |
2008年 | 1751篇 |
2007年 | 1699篇 |
2006年 | 1561篇 |
2005年 | 1295篇 |
2004年 | 1328篇 |
2003年 | 1280篇 |
2002年 | 1208篇 |
2001年 | 1952篇 |
2000年 | 1821篇 |
1999年 | 1361篇 |
1998年 | 630篇 |
1997年 | 544篇 |
1996年 | 619篇 |
1995年 | 565篇 |
1994年 | 559篇 |
1993年 | 535篇 |
1992年 | 1116篇 |
1991年 | 1030篇 |
1990年 | 1014篇 |
1989年 | 974篇 |
1988年 | 948篇 |
1987年 | 874篇 |
1986年 | 869篇 |
1985年 | 934篇 |
1984年 | 741篇 |
1983年 | 646篇 |
1982年 | 494篇 |
1979年 | 771篇 |
1978年 | 578篇 |
1975年 | 607篇 |
1974年 | 698篇 |
1973年 | 712篇 |
1972年 | 606篇 |
1971年 | 568篇 |
1970年 | 489篇 |
1968年 | 616篇 |
1967年 | 538篇 |
1966年 | 513篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Speech nonfluency in response to questions about the marital relationship was used to assess anxiety. Subjects were 31 husbands and 31 wives, all white, college educated, from middle- to lower-middle-class families, and ranging from 20 to 30 years of age. Three types of nonfluencies were coded: filled pauses, unfilled pauses, and repetitions. Speech-disturbance ratios were computed by dividing the sum of speech nonfluencies by the total words spoken. The results support the notion that some issues within marriage are more sensitive and/or problematic than others, and that, in an interview situation, gender interacts with question content in the production of nonfluencies. 相似文献
102.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
103.
Effects of Social Demand on Reports of Self-Monitored Blood Glucose in Adolescents with Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Alan M. Delamater Steven M. Kurtz Neil H. White Julio V. Santiago 《Journal of applied social psychology》1988,18(6):491-502
The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that social demand could substantially affect reports of self-monitored blood glucose (BG) in adolescents with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Of 34 patients initially enrolled in the study, 10 were excluded because they did not bring any BG records with them to an outpatient clinic appointment. The remaining 24 patients were randomly assigned to either a low or high social demand condition that provided instructions for monitoring of BG for the week following the appointment. The subjects' BG records were quantified to provide frequency of measurement and mean reported BG for the week prior to and after the clinic visit. Five subjects did not return their BG records for the week following the intervention. The analyses were therefore based on the 19 subjects from whom complete records were obtained. The 12 subjects in the low social demand group and 7 subjects in the high social demand group were equivalent with regard to age, duration of diabetes, socioeconomic status, and glycosylated hemoglobin. Frequency of BG measurement was similar in both groups during both weeks. The mean BG value reported in the week prior to intervention was similar for the groups. However, analyses of the post-intervention BGs revealed that subjects in the low-demand group reported significantly higher BGs compared to pre-intervention and to subjects in the high-demand group. These findings suggest that self-monitoring and reporting of BG is a social behavior that is affected by the demand characteristics of the interpersonal patient-health provider relationship. Because optimal treatment planning for individuals with diabetes requires accurate BG records, care must be taken to interpret them in light of the social demand characteristics associated with clinical assessment. 相似文献
104.
105.
Thirty pediatricians and 30 parents were interviewed on (a) how and when children develop concepts about death, (b) children's emotional responses to the thought of death, (c) the possibility of dialogue between adults and children about death, and (d) differences in healthy and seriously ill children's conceptualization of death. Pediatricians were no different than parents in their opinions. Moreover, in some cases, less educated parents and older doctors tended to hold opinions that were even farther from reality. Overall, the results suggest that children, healthy or sick, are left alone with the problem of death and are not helped either by their parents or by their doctors. 相似文献
106.
Training in systemic therapy necessitates a structured approach to teaching systemic thinking. The Systemic Therapy Sessions Summary Form, a record-keeping method, is presented as a tool for enhancing training in systemic therapy. The purpose of this form is to provide the structure for the trainee to develop circular hypotheses, to connect reflexively the assessment and intervention components of therapy, and to maintain a sense of the evolving nature of therapy across sessions. A case example illustrates the use of this record-keeping format. 相似文献
107.
This article presents a conceptual framework for individual treatment with chronic anorectic patients. In the first part of the article, the six-stage model for the anorectic process developed over the past years by M. Selvini-Palazzoli and her team is briefly explained. After presenting the model, we then discuss some related concepts, namely, "family game," "rule," and "strategy." Finally, the therapeutic technique in individual treatment is discussed. Two basic assumptions underlie this technique: first, the therapeutic relationship is instrumental, its main purpose being to improve the patient's personal relations outside therapy; second, the focus of the treatment is on the strategic incapacity of the patient who is caught up in and unable to cope with the "concealed family game." 相似文献
108.
The present study demonstrates the safety and effectiveness of an outpatient program designed to respond to adolescent-precipitated crises by mobilizing and restructuring the family's kinship system. Families of 75 adolescents at risk for hospitalization were followed up to 24 months after treatment with Systemic Crisis Intervention. Measures of offspring and family functioning, suicidal behavior, institutional use, and treatment costs are presented. Results clearly demonstrate the safety, effectiveness, and economic viability of Systemic Crisis Intervention. 相似文献
109.
Therapists have a mandate to bring about change, and family therapists appear to have unusual leverage to fullfill such a mandate, that is, to facilitate changes rapidly and effectively. Because and in spite of this mandate, the very notion of change deserves critical reflection. The history of ideas and our clinical experience teach us that changes may be problematic. They may come too fast or too slowly, may endure or fade away, may turn out to be desirable or undesirable, equitable or inequitable, and may, when further consequences and larger systems levels are taken into account, be beneficial or disastrous. This essay examines some of the problematical and contradictory aspects of change and delineates two perspectives on change--systemic optimism and systemic pessimism. 相似文献
110.
M Baum 《Behaviour research and therapy》1988,26(2):185-186