全文获取类型
收费全文 | 420篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
431篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 55篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1932年 | 1篇 |
1921年 | 1篇 |
1920年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有431条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Humans accomplish much of what they do in collaboration with others. In ontogeny, children's earliest abilities to collaborate develop in two basic steps. First, 1- and 2-year-olds learn to form with others joint goals and joint attention--which include an understanding of the individual roles and perspectives involved. Second, as they approach their third birthdays, children's collaborative interactions with others take on a more normative dimension involving obligations to the partner. In addition, their cognitive abilities to conceptualize simultaneously both their own role and perspective along with those of the other develop considerably as well. This form of collaborative interaction is underlain by species-unique skills and motivations for shared intentionality that make possible, ultimately, such things as complex cultural institutions. 相似文献
252.
Thirty-two infants participating in a longitudinal study of the effects of premature birth on sucking and breathing were tested at 3 months post term. They were assigned at a previous test to either a healthy full-term, healthy preterm, or high risk preterm group on the basis of birth weight, postconceptional age at birth, and perinatal medical status. Positive and negative sucking pressure as well as chest and abdominal breathing movements were recorded during nutritive sucking. The high risk preterms used a simpler and more stable pattern of coordination between sucking and breathing, were more likely to interrupt breathing at milk onset, and produced longer phase lags between positive and negative sucking pressure. 相似文献
254.
255.
256.
This paper surveys the current philosophical discussion of the ethics of risk imposition, placing it in the context of relevant work in psychology, economics and social theory. The central philosophical problem starts from the observation that it is not practically possible to assign people individual rights not to be exposed to risk, as virtually all activity imposes some risk on others. This is the ‘problem of paralysis’. However, the obvious alternative theory that exposure to risk is justified when its total benefits exceed its total costs faces the standard distributional challenges of consequentialism. Forms of contractualism have been proposed as a solution, but how exactly such theories can be formulated remains problematic, especially when confronted with the difficult cases of mass, novel, risk such as climate change. 相似文献
257.
258.
An online version of a Picture Story Exercise (PSE) was administered to 180 participants to assess implicit motives for achievement,
affiliation, and power. First, data were analyzed with respect to different aspects of participant behavior (dropout, writing
time, story length, setting). Second, online data were compared with data collected in the laboratory (n = 93). Results regarding motive density, internal consistency, picture profiles, and position effects were comparable among
samples and consistent with previous research (Pang and Schultheiss in J Person Assess 85:280–294, 2005; Schultheiss and Brunstein in J Person Assess 77: 71–86, 2001). In addition, online participants completed self-report measures assessing explicit motives and the Big Five personality
factors. Explicit motives showed no substantial overlap with PSE motive scores, replicating previous research. Personality
factors revealed gender differences in correlates: men with a high implicit power motive reported less agreeableness and conscientiousness.
Results suggest that administering the PSE online yields reliable and valid data. 相似文献
259.
Cropping is the central act of photography, the viewfinder of a camera being used to crop a portion of the visual world which is then surrounded with a frame. Six studies are described which show that the act of cropping is carried out reliably and confidently by both expert and non-expert participants. Two studies confirm that some croppers are better croppers than others, their cropped images being preferred aesthetically over the croppings of less-good croppers. Colour had little impact on cropping decisions, whereas thresholded monochrome images ('Mooney images') dramatically altered crop positions. That suggests that cropping can be driven top-down, by the meaning of objects in photographs, but the fact that the Mooneyised images are still cropped consistently, suggests that image structure, perhaps in the form of low-level image properties, may still be important. Experts crop pictures differently from non-experts, and they take longer, viewing a wider range of possible crops, pausing longer to assess crops, and using more formal terminology when reflecting on their cropping decisions. Experts' crops are not, however, preferred more, either by non-expert viewers or by expert viewers. Cropping, it is suggested, is an ideal paradigm for experimental aesthetics, allowing precise experimental control with Fechner's Method of Production, a technique which normally is not easy to use. 相似文献
260.
There is accumulating evidence that individual differences in stress reactivity contribute to the risk for stress-related disease. However, the assessment of stress reactivity remains challenging, and there is a relative lack of questionnaires reliably assessing this construct. We here present the Perceived Stress Reactivity Scale (PSRS), a 23-item questionnaire with 5 subscales and 1 overall scale, based on an existing German-language instrument. Perceived stress reactivity and related constructs were assessed in N = 2,040 participants from the United Kingdom, the United States, and Germany. The 5-factor structure of the PSRS was found to be similar in the 3 countries. In the U.S. sample the questionnaire was applied using 2 modes of administration (paper-pencil and computerized), and measures were repeated after 4 weeks. Measurement invariance analyses demonstrated full invariance across mode of administration and partial invariance across gender and countries. Scale scores differed between countries and genders, with women scoring higher on most scales. Overall, reliability analysis suggested good stability of PSRS scores over a 4-week period, and validation analysis showed expected associations with related constructs such as self-efficacy, neuroticism, chronic stress, and perceived stress. Perceived stress reactivity was also associated with depressive symptoms and sleep. These associations were particularly strong when individuals scoring high on perceived stress reactivity were exposed to chronic stress. In sum, our findings suggest that the PSRS is a useful and easy-to-administer instrument to assess perceived stress reactivity. 相似文献