全文获取类型
收费全文 | 239篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
247篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 27篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有247条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
Juliane Muehlhaus Stefan Heim Olga Sachs Frank Schneider Ute Habel Katharina Sass 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2013,42(1):37-49
The aim of the present study was to investigate the impact of part-whole (e.g., car-motor) and functional associations (e.g., car-garage) on single word (Experiment 1) and sentence production (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, a classical picture-word task was used. In Experiment 2, the same stimuli and distractors were embedded into a sentence. The relation between target and distractor was either part-whole, functional or unrelated. At single word level, part-whole and functional relations facilitate naming. Additionally, the facilitation effect was stronger for part-whole in comparison to functional associations. During sentence production, facilitation shifted to interference. The difference between both relations disappeared. The findings of the different effects between functional and part-whole associations depend on the length of utterances and highlight the divergent impact of associations. The differences between part-whole and functional associations in single word production might reflect a differential organization of associative links at the conceptual level. In contrast, during sentence production the syntactic processing at the lexical level seem to be more important than types of semantic associations at the conceptual level. 相似文献
222.
Dr. Katharina Lau Dipl.-Psych. Bernd Löwe Gernot Langs Katharina Voigt 《Psychotherapeut》2013,58(6):545-551
Background
The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5 (DSM-5) includes a revision of the DSM-IV criteria for somatoform disorders. The aim of the current work was to investigate whether (a) patients with DSM-IV diagnoses of somatization disorder, pain disorder and hypochondriasis and (b) whether patients categorized as having the DSM-5 somatic symptom disorder and illness anxiety disorder differ with respect to illness anxiety and cognition regarding somatic symptoms.Material and methods
The data from 269 inpatients from the psychosomatic clinic Schön Klinik Bad Bramstedt were used. Somatoform disorders were diagnosed using the German version of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID).Results
Patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of hypochondriasis differed in illness anxiety and catastrophizing interpretation of somatic symptoms compared to patients with other somatoform disorders. Patients with illness anxiety disorder differed in the catastrophizing interpretation of physical symptoms, autonomic sensations, bodily weakness and intolerance of physical complaints compared to patients with somatic symptom disorder.Conclusion
The present results indicate that illness anxiety and a catastrophizing interpretation of somatic symptoms play a fundamental role in patients with somatoform disorders. Therefore, psychotherapy should address illness anxiety and health-related concerns in all patients with somatoform disorders. 相似文献223.
224.
Bilingualism research has established language non-selective lexical access in comprehension. However, the evidence for such an effect in production remains sparse and its neural time-course has not yet been investigated. We demonstrate that German-English bilinguals performing a simple picture-naming task exclusively in English spontaneously access the phonological form of –unproduced– German words. Participants were asked to produce English adjective-noun sequences describing the colour and identity of familiar objects presented as line drawings. We associated adjective and picture names such that their onsets phonologically overlapped in English (e.g., green goat), in German through translation (e.g., blue flower – ‘blaue Blume’), or in neither language. As expected, phonological priming in English modulated event-related brain potentials over the frontocentral scalp region from around 440 ms after picture onset. Phonological priming in German was detectable even earlier, from 300 ms, even though German was never produced and in the absence of an interaction between language and phonological repetition priming at any point in time. Overall, these results establish the existence of non-selective access to phonological representations of the two languages in the domain of speech production. 相似文献
225.
Spalek K Franck J Schriefers H Frauenfelder UH 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2008,37(6):419-442
Two experiments investigate whether native speakers of French can use a noun’s phonological ending to retrieve its gender
and that of a gender-marked element. In Experiment 1, participants performed a gender decision task on the noun’s gender-marked
determiner for auditorily presented nouns. Noun endings with high predictive values were selected. The noun stimuli could
either belong to the gender class predicted by their ending (congruent) or they could belong to the gender class that was
different from the predicted gender (incongruent). Gender decisions were made significantly faster for congruent nouns than
for incongruent nouns, relative to a (lexical decision) baseline task. In Experiment 2, participants named pictures of the
same materials as used in Experiment 1 with noun phrases consisting of a gender-marked determiner, a gender-marked adjective
and a noun. In this Experiment, no effect of congruency, relative to a (bare noun naming) baseline task, was observed. Thus,
the results show an effect of phonological information on the retrieval of gender-marked elements in spoken word recognition,
but not in word production. 相似文献
226.
This paper explores why people identify with social groups and what this identification signifies for their sense of self,
status in society and intergroup conflict. We describe various theories of social identity to elucidate ways in which individuals
can negotiate their different social identities, and what this means for intergroup relations. We consider the implications
for both majority and minority group members, and those from high and low status groups. We show that social identification
is an essential part of an individual’s social existence, and that such identification is inextricably related to intergroup
conflict. While overarching common identities have been hailed as a possible panacea for conflict, we demonstrate that such
identities have differential effects for minority and majority group members. There is a serious tension between the assimilationist
preference that the majority wishes for minority members to adopt, and the integrationist position that the minority group
themselves prefer. We conclude with a call to focus research efforts on how to balance the needs of the many and the few in
pluralist and unequal societies. 相似文献
227.
Journal for General Philosophy of Science - In logic books ordinary language examples are often thought to be easier to understand, as it is presupposed or suggested, that their logical structure... 相似文献
228.
Katharina Sobota 《Argumentation》1991,5(3):275-282
Usually, rhetoric is supposed to provide a model of law which describes legal interactions as volitive, arbitrary, and void of any system. However, the Mainz School, founded by Theodor Viehweg, proves the contrary: without referring to any metaphysics, such as consensus or auditoire universel, it seeks to discover the systematic character and the more or less flexible structures of legal rhetoric. 相似文献
229.
Borden’s (1979, 1980) hypothesis that speakers with vulnerable speech systems rely more heavily on feedback monitoring than do speakers with less vulnerable systems was investigated. The second language (L2) of a speaker is vulnerable, in comparison with the native language, so alteration to feedback should have a detrimental effect on it, according to this hypothesis. Here, we specifically examined whether altered auditory feedback has an effect on accent strength when speakers speak L2. There were three stages in the experiment. First, 6 German speakers who were fluent in English (their L2) were recorded under six conditions—normal listening, amplified voice level, voice shifted in frequency, delayed auditory feedback, and slowed and accelerated speech rate conditions. Second, judges were trained to rate accent strength. Training was assessed by whether it was successful in separating German speakers speaking English from native English speakers, also speaking English. In the final stage, the judges ranked recordings of each speaker from the first stage as to increasing strength of German accent. The results show that accents were more pronounced under frequency-shifted and delayed auditory feedback conditions than under normal or amplified feedback conditions. Control tests were done to ensure that listeners were judging accent, rather than fluency changes caused by altered auditory feedback. The findings are discussed in terms of Borden’s hypothesis and other accounts about why altered auditory feedback disrupts speech control. 相似文献
230.
Steffen Moritz Katharina Voigt G. Marina Arzola Christian Otte 《Memory (Hove, England)》2013,21(8):810-820
Since the 1980s a large body of empirical effort has been devoted to mood-congruent memory (MCM) biases in clinical depression. Whereas there is broad, albeit not unequivocal, evidence that depressive patients retain negative-valenced memory items better than neutral material, few studies have investigated false memories in depression. In a pilot study we gathered support for both enhanced true and false memory for emotional material in depression. The present study aimed to extend these preliminary findings. In view of investigations suggesting that arousing and meaningful stimuli have facilitated access to memory, personal salience was considered a moderator for MCM. In the present study 21 depressed and 22 healthy participants were presented six false memory lists dealing with neutral, negative, and positive themes. At recognition, each item had to be appraised for its degree of valence subsequent to an old–new judgement. Pre-categorised and subjective valence did not discriminate groups. However, relative to controls depressed patients showed both more veridical as well as false recognition for items that concurrently elicited higher salience ratings in patients. In contrast, group differences in recognition performance did not significantly affect salience ratings. Results indicate that salience modulates MCM and may account for discrepancies in the literature. 相似文献