全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1004篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1022篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 38篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 143篇 |
2012年 | 34篇 |
2011年 | 43篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 30篇 |
2007年 | 39篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 31篇 |
2004年 | 32篇 |
2003年 | 29篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1937年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1022条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Visual orienting has typically been characterized using simple displays—for example, displays with a static target placed on a homogeneous background. In the present study, visual orienting was investigated using a dynamic broadband (1/f) noise display that should mimic a more naturalistic setting and that should allow saccadic orienting experiments to be performed with fewer constraints. In Experiment 1, it was shown that the noise movie contains gaze-attracting features that are almost as distinct as the ones measured for (static) real-word scenes. The movie can therefore serve as a strong distractor. In Experiment 2, observers carried out a luminance target search that showed that saccadic amplitude errors were substantially higher (18%) than the ones measured in simple displays. That error is certainly one of the primary factors making gaze-fixation prediction in complex scenes difficult. Supplemental figures for this study may be downloaded from http://app.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
52.
Karl Halvor Teigen 《Current Psychology》1985,4(1):3-16
When people are asked to rate verbal material (texts, statements, and statements forming part of a text) according to informativeness,
their judgments will to a large extent depend upon how much they already know about the subject, and how novel the communication
is to them. This will in some cases make them stress novelty and in other cases familiarity as the most important determinants
for expected or perceived informativeness. It is argued that these apparently contradictory trends are reconcilable by a propositional
(subject-predicate) model of information, which presupposes an identifiablesubject of the communication (“what it is all about”), as well as something to be predicated about this subject, topic, or theme.
This kind of information structure allows the communication to contain both novel and familiar elements at the same time,
with informativeness being at a peak when something quite new and unexpected is told about a familiar subject, or when a new
subject is made familiar (i.e., satisfactorily explained) to the person.
This article was written when the author was on sabbatical leave at the University of Leicester.
The study was supported by a grant from the Norwegian Research Council for Science and the Humanities. 相似文献
53.
Steve Petersen 《Synthese》2013,190(6):1173-1184
Standard epistemology takes it for granted that there is a special kind of value: epistemic value. This claim does not seem to sit well with act utilitarianism, however, since it holds that only welfare is of real value. I first develop a particularly utilitarian sense of “epistemic value”, according to which it is closely analogous to the nature of financial value. I then demonstrate the promise this approach has for two current puzzles in the intersection of epistemology and value theory: first, the problem of why knowledge is better than mere true belief, and second, the relation between epistemic justification and responsibility. 相似文献
54.
Although fluid intelligence and complex learning are conceptualized differently and assessed by apparently different measures, both theoretical accounts and empirical evidence suggest a relationship between the two constructs. In this study, major working memory aspects including the storage capacity and executive attention were proposed to account for the relationship between fluid intelligence and complex learning. A sample of 184 participants completed fluid intelligence and complex learning scales, as well as working memory measures that each included two or three treatment levels differing in the demands on capacity or executive control. The differences among the treatment levels provided a favorable precondition for employing fixed-links models to separate the core processes of storage capacity or executive attention from the auxiliary processes. Results indicated that both storage capacity and executive attention contributed significantly to fluid intelligence and complex learning. A further analysis showed that the two working memory aspects, particularly the storage capacity, accounted for most of the shared variance between fluid intelligence and complex learning. 相似文献
55.
Nicole R. Guajardo Rachel Petersen Timothy R. Marshall 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(3):225-252
The authors examined effects of feedback and explanation on false belief performance. Thirty-three children (42–54 months; 15 girls, 18 boys) were randomly assigned to four treatment conditions: explanation, feedback, feedback researcher explains, and feedback child explains. Children completed false belief tasks during pretraining, 8 training sessions, and posttraining across 6 weeks. Language comprehension was assessed at pretraining. The authors hypothesized that children would improve most when training involved feedback and explanation. Generalized estimating equations modeling was used to analyze the data. Children who received feedback and generated explanations for characters’ false beliefs improved across training sessions more so than children in other conditions. Children's explanations for false beliefs also were explored. Implications of the findings are discussed. 相似文献
56.
57.
Karl B. Frank 《International journal of group psychotherapy》2013,63(4):395-396
AbstractGroup psychotherapists in their everyday practice confront a series of ethical problems, some of which rise to the level of ethical dilemmas. This two-part special series will address how the group psychotherapist can address these problems and dilemmas in a way that leads to an ethical course of conduct. This article introduces the series by examining ethical principles and decision-making processes that are relevant to the wide range of issues that confront the group psychotherapist. The article also considers the person of the group psychotherapist him or herself and notes that certain personal qualities might create a foundation for ethical thinking and behavior. 相似文献
58.
59.
Willa S. Bass Karl M. Oswald 《Advances in cognitive psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw》2014,10(2):49-58
Proactive interferencebuilds up with exposure to multiple lists of similar items
with a resulting reduction in recall. This study examined the effectiveness of
using a proactive strategy of the method of loci to reduce proactive
interference in a list recall paradigm of categorically similar words. While all
participants reported using some form of strategy to recall list words, this
study demonstrated that young adults were able to proactively use the method of
loci after 25 min of instruction to reduce proactive interference as compared
with other personal spontaneous strategies. The implications of this study are
that top-down proactive strategies such as the method of loci can significantly
reduce proactive interference, and that the use of image and sequence or
location are especially useful in this regard. 相似文献
60.
Kurland J Naeser MA Baker EH Doron K Martin PI Seekins HE Bogdan A Renshaw P Yurgelun-Todd D 《Behavioural neurology》2004,15(3-4):87-97
Cortical reorganization in poststroke aphasia is not well understood. Few studies have investigated neural mechanisms underlying language recovery in severe aphasia patients, who are typically viewed as having a poor prognosis for language recovery. Although test-retest reliability is routinely demonstrated during collection of language data in single-subject aphasia research, this is rarely examined in fMRI studies investigating the underlying neural mechanisms in aphasia recovery. The purpose of this study was to acquire fMRI test-retest data examining semantic decisions both within and between two aphasia patients. Functional MRI was utilized to image individuals with chronic, moderate-severe nonfluent aphasia during nonverbal, yes/no button-box semantic judgments of iconic sentences presented in the Computer-assisted Visual Communication (C-ViC) program. We investigated the critical issue of intra-subject reliability by exploring similarities and differences in regions of activation during participants' performance of identical tasks twice on the same day. Each participant demonstrated high intra-subject reliability, with response decrements typical of task familiarity. Differences between participants included greater left hemisphere perilesional activation in the individual with better response to C-ViC training. This study provides fMRI reliability in chronic nonfluent aphasia, and adds to evidence supporting differences in individual cortical reorganization in aphasia recovery. 相似文献