首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2908篇
  免费   134篇
  3042篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   26篇
  2020年   51篇
  2019年   68篇
  2018年   79篇
  2017年   85篇
  2016年   75篇
  2015年   84篇
  2014年   71篇
  2013年   335篇
  2012年   109篇
  2011年   133篇
  2010年   95篇
  2009年   85篇
  2008年   118篇
  2007年   115篇
  2006年   98篇
  2005年   110篇
  2004年   111篇
  2003年   112篇
  2002年   104篇
  2001年   53篇
  2000年   38篇
  1999年   42篇
  1998年   60篇
  1997年   51篇
  1996年   39篇
  1995年   21篇
  1994年   34篇
  1993年   44篇
  1992年   20篇
  1991年   21篇
  1990年   24篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   20篇
  1987年   25篇
  1986年   28篇
  1985年   38篇
  1984年   26篇
  1983年   25篇
  1982年   22篇
  1981年   32篇
  1980年   32篇
  1979年   16篇
  1978年   22篇
  1977年   23篇
  1976年   23篇
  1974年   14篇
  1973年   15篇
  1971年   16篇
排序方式: 共有3042条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
The Rokeach Dogmatism Scale and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire were correlated with educator-reported use of corporal punishment. Respondents were from a medium-size school system in Tennessee. Results indicated that closed-mindedness, as measured by the Rokeach scale, and Neuroticism, as measured by the Eysenck Scale, were highly correlated with reported use of corporal punishment. Of the remaining two dimensions of personality measured by the Eysenck questionnaire, Extraversion was found to be moderately correlated with reported use of corporal punishment while Psychoticism showed no significant relationship. A significant negative correlation was found between years of experience in teaching and use of corporal punishment. Frequency of physical punishment used on an educator when he/she was in grades K-12 was positively correlated with reported use of corporal punishment. Heavy users of corporal punishment tended to be relatively inexperienced, close-minded, neurotic, and impulsive as compared to their peers who did not use corporal punishment.  相似文献   
72.
73.
Potential members of the International Fluency Association (IFA) were polled regarding their fluency interests and perceived priorities for the organization during the year it was founded (in 1990). Results from 201 respondents indicated that a need for the IFA existed among researchers, clinicians, and representatives of self-help organizations for people who stutter. Improving the quality of treatment for those who stutter was considered the highest priority by all three groups. Researchers were of the opinion that the Journal of Fluency Disorders should be of higher quality than it had been in the past, and clinicians were most interested in reviews of new and relevant materials.  相似文献   
74.
75.
In three experiments, subjects imposed sentence in a criminal case before and after reading the harsh sentence imposed by the trial judge. They then read and imposed sentence in another case. In one experimental condition the subjects anticipated learning the judge's sentence in Case 2; in the other condition they were told not to anticipate learning the judge's sentence. Control subjects made their judgments without ever seeing the judge's sentence. Direct influence was the change in sentences within cases; indirect influence was the difference between initial sentences in Cases 1 and 2. Though direct influence was minimal and not significant, indirect influence was substantial, and was greater in the anticipation than no-anticipation than control conditions, indicating that both modification of the judgmental process and anticipatory conformity were operating. Indirect influence may then reflect a change of either type, depending on a number of methodological and other factors.  相似文献   
76.
77.
78.
In a two-stimulus matching paradigm requiring “same” and “different” responses to simple geometric dimensions, the successive stimulus presentation mode with several retention intervals was compared to a simultaneous stimulus presentation mode. For the simultaneous condition, the data suggested a wholistic template matching process, but in none of the successive conditions was template matching indicated. The retention interval of the successive condition allows the subject to analyze out the target dimension from the first two-dimensional stimulus. The ability to anticipate the value of the target dimension in the second stimulus could also influence the representation of the first stimulus. The findings shed some light upon the perceptual interactions between dimensions that constitute the stimulus. In particular, an outside-to-inside order of iconic scanning appeared to be supported by the data.  相似文献   
79.
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号