首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   5220篇
  免费   1476篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2023年   13篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   101篇
  2020年   115篇
  2019年   427篇
  2018年   328篇
  2017年   441篇
  2016年   425篇
  2015年   398篇
  2014年   351篇
  2013年   719篇
  2012年   345篇
  2011年   290篇
  2010年   307篇
  2009年   271篇
  2008年   283篇
  2007年   199篇
  2006年   178篇
  2005年   192篇
  2004年   175篇
  2003年   176篇
  2002年   180篇
  2001年   115篇
  2000年   91篇
  1999年   68篇
  1998年   41篇
  1997年   49篇
  1996年   25篇
  1995年   17篇
  1994年   31篇
  1993年   35篇
  1992年   15篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   18篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   22篇
  1985年   24篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   19篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   20篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   6篇
  1973年   5篇
  1950年   5篇
排序方式: 共有6700条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Long-term retention of operant footkicking acquired in the mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm was assessed as a function of the distribution of training time. In the first study, 3-month-old infants were trained for either one 18-min session or for two 9-min or three 6-min sessions separated by 24-hr intervals. All infants exhibited retention during a test administered immediately after training, but only those trained in a single session continued to perform the conditioned response during cued-recall tests 7 or 14 days later. Infants trained in three sessions showed no evidence of remembering the contingency even after a week. A warm-up decrement, seen in the day-to-day performance of infants in the distributed conditions, was eliminated in the second study by the interpolation of a nonreinforcement period at the outset of daily sessions. This procedure also enhanced long-term retention such that infants trained in three 6-min session now remembered the contingency for 14 days and did not differ from infants who had received a single 18-min session. Whether distributed training facilitates or impairs long-term retention appears to depend on the opportunity for infants to acquire a sufficient number (or kind) of effective retrieval cues during original learning.  相似文献   
72.
The Rokeach Dogmatism Scale and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire were correlated with educator-reported use of corporal punishment. Respondents were from a medium-size school system in Tennessee. Results indicated that closed-mindedness, as measured by the Rokeach scale, and Neuroticism, as measured by the Eysenck Scale, were highly correlated with reported use of corporal punishment. Of the remaining two dimensions of personality measured by the Eysenck questionnaire, Extraversion was found to be moderately correlated with reported use of corporal punishment while Psychoticism showed no significant relationship. A significant negative correlation was found between years of experience in teaching and use of corporal punishment. Frequency of physical punishment used on an educator when he/she was in grades K-12 was positively correlated with reported use of corporal punishment. Heavy users of corporal punishment tended to be relatively inexperienced, close-minded, neurotic, and impulsive as compared to their peers who did not use corporal punishment.  相似文献   
73.
74.
75.
A FAITH FOR ALL SEASONS:ISLAM AND WESTERN MODERNITY. By Shabbir Akhtar. London: Bellew Publishing, 1990. 251pp. Hb. £25.00. ISBN 0–947792–41–4.  相似文献   
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号