全文获取类型
收费全文 | 626篇 |
免费 | 84篇 |
国内免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
741篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 49篇 |
2005年 | 47篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有741条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Humans are limited in their ability to maintain multiple attentional foci. In attentive tracking of moving objects, performance
declines as the number of tracked targets increases. Previous studies have interpreted such reduction in terms of a limit
in the number of attentional foci. However, increasing the number of targets usually reduces spatial separation among different
targets. In this study, we examine the role of target spatial separation in maintaining multiple attentional foci. Results
from a multiple-object tracking task show that tracking accuracy deteriorates as the spatial separation between targets decreases.
We propose that local interaction between nearby attentional foci modulates the resolution of attention, and that capacity
limitation from attentive tracking originates in part from limitations in maintaining critical spacing among multiple attentional
foci. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that tracking performance is limited not primarily by a number of
locations, but by factors such as the spacing and speed of the targets and distractors. 相似文献
52.
Yuhong Jiang Marvin M. Chun 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2001,54(4):1105-1124
The effect of selective attention on implicit learning was tested in four experiments using the contextual cueing paradigm (Chun & Jiang, 1998, 1999). Observers performed visual search through items presented in an attended colour (e.g., red) and an ignored colour (e.g., green). When the spatial configuration of items in the attended colour was invariant and was consistently paired with a target location, visual search was facilitated, showing contextual cueing (Experiments 1, 3, and 4). In contrast, repeating and pairing the configuration of the ignored items with the target location resulted in no contextual cueing (Experiments 2 and 4). We conclude that implicit learning is robust only when relevant, predictive information is selectively attended. 相似文献
53.
Makovski T Sussman R Jiang YV 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2008,34(2):369-380
Given a changing visual environment, and the limited capacity of visual working memory (VWM), the contents of VWM must be in constant flux. Using a change detection task, the authors show that VWM is subject to obligatory updating in the face of new information. Change detection performance is enhanced when the item that may change is retrospectively cued 1 s after memory encoding and 0.5 s before testing. The retro-cue benefit cannot be explained by memory decay or by a reduction in interference from other items held in VWM. Rather, orienting attention to a single memory item makes VWM more resistant to interference from the test probe. The authors conclude that the content of VWM is volatile unless it receives focused attention, and that the standard change detection task underestimates VWM capacity. 相似文献
54.
Previous research has shown that visual search performance is modulated by the current contents in visual working memory (VWM), even when the contents of VWM are irrelevant to the search task. For example, visual search is faster when the target--rather than a distractor--is surrounded by a shape currently held in VWM. This study uses the modulation of visual search by VWM to investigate properties of VWM. Participants wereasked to remember the color or the shape of novel polygons whose "goodness" of figure varied according to Garner's (1962) rotation and reflection transformation principle. During the memory retention interval, participants searched for a tilted line among vertical lines embedded inside colored polygons. Search was faster when the target--rather than a distractor--was enclosed by the remembered polygons. The congruity effect diminished with increasing memory load and decreasing figure goodness. We conclude that congruity effects in visual search can indirectly assess VWM representation strength. 相似文献
55.
56.
57.
Yang Jiang Allan J. Pantle Leonard S. Mark 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1998,60(2):275-286
Five experiments were designed to determine whether a rotating, transparent 3-D cloud of dots (simulated sphere) could influence the perceived direction of rotation of a subsequent sphere. Experiment 1 established conditions under which the direction of rotation of a virtual sphere was perceived unambiguously. When a near-far luminance difference and perspective depth cues were present, observers consistently saw the sphere rotate in the intended direction. In Experiment 2, a near-far luminance difference was used to create an unambiguous rotation sequence that was followed by a directionally ambiguous rotation sequence that lacked both the near-far luminance cue and the perspective cue. Observers consistently saw the second sequence as rotating in the same direction as the first, indicating the presence of 3-D visual inertia. Experiment 3 showed that 3-D visual inertia was sufficiently powerful to bias the perceived direction of a rotation sequence made unambiguous by a near-far luminance cue. Experiment 5 showed that 3-D visual inertia could be obtained using an occlusion depth cue to create an unambiguous inertia-inducing sequence. Finally, Experiments 2, 4, and 5 all revealed a fast-decay phase of inertia that lasted for approximately 800 msec, followed by an asymptotic phase that lasted for periods as long as 1,600 msec. The implications of these findings are examined with respect to motion mechanisms of 3-D visual inertia. 相似文献
58.
Jiang Luo 《Psychology & health》2013,28(8):963-984
The purpose of the present study was to investigate how optimists process health-related information. Sixty-five young adults (ages 18–35) reported skin cancer-related knowledge and behaviors, and read slides of information on skin and skin cancer. Visual attention to the slides was recorded using eye tracking, and their memory for the information was measured. Additionally, participants’ self-reported skin cancer-relevant behavior was assessed prospectively in the months following the lab component of the study. Results show that individuals low in dispositional optimism or high in health-related optimism paid more attention when they were at high objective risk of developing skin cancer; and individuals high in dispositional optimism or high in health-related optimism were more likely to perform adaptive, health-promoting behaviors. In addition, optimistic beliefs were found not to be related with unrealistic optimism. Dispositional and health-related optimism therefore appear to predict health-related cognition and behavior in distinct ways. 相似文献
59.
60.
该研究探讨了我国矿工群体不安全心理的状况和表现,编订与矿工职业特点相适应的不安全心理测量工具.方法:从矿工不安全心理主题入手,以文献研究为基础,结合访谈和开放式问卷所获资料,并对2033名矿工进行施测,对调查数据进行探索性因素分析和验证性因素分析,形成正式问卷.结果:探索性因素分析表明,矿工不安全心理包括安全无奈感、逆反心理、麻痹心理、临时心理四个方面;验证性因素分析结果表明构想模型拟合较好,问卷的信效度均达到心理测量学的要求.结论:矿工不安全心理问卷可以作为研究我国矿工不安全心理的一个有效测量工具. 相似文献