全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1258篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
1322篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 15篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 134篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 53篇 |
2010年 | 41篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 57篇 |
2007年 | 56篇 |
2006年 | 48篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 35篇 |
2002年 | 25篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 15篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 17篇 |
1983年 | 24篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1965年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1322条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
202.
203.
Johannes Seehusen Filippo Cordaro Tim Wildschut Constantine Sedikides Clay Routledge Ginette C. Blackhart Kai Epstude Ad J.J.M. Vingerhoets 《Personality and individual differences》2013
Who is the nostalgia-prone person? The ‘sociality view’ sees an individual who frequently recalls meaningful memories rich in social content. The ‘maladaptation view’ sees an emotionally unstable, neurotic individual. In four studies, we integrated these contrasting views. We hypothesized that the link between neuroticism and nostalgia proneness arises because (a) neuroticism is associated with the need to belong and (b) the need to belong triggers nostalgia, with its abundant social content. Consistent with this hypothesis, Studies 1–2 found that the correlation between neuroticism and nostalgia proneness was eliminated when controlling for the need to belong. The need to belong predicted increased nostalgia proneness, above and beyond neuroticism. Specifically, Study 2 revealed that a deficit-reduction (rather than growth) belongingness orientation predicted increased nostalgia proneness. When the role of this deficit-reduction belongingness orientation was controlled, the positive correlation between neuroticism and nostalgia disappeared. Studies 3–4 showed that experimental inductions of a belongingness deficit augmented nostalgia, providing support for its compensatory role. 相似文献
204.
This article describes the initial development of a new measure of individual polychronicity, a construct indicating a preference for multitasking as opposed to performing only one task at a time. Following a brief review of past definitional issues, a revised definition is offered and a new measure of polychronicity—the Multitasking Preference Inventory (MPI)—is created based on this definition. Three studies describe the creation, pilot testing, and psychometric refinement of an initial pool of items (Study 1); a convergent and discriminant validity analysis (Study 2); and initial findings with respect to the criterion-related validity of scores on the MPI (Study 3). Together, these findings provide preliminary evidence for the reliability of scores on the MPI and the validity of the MPI in predicting variables relevant to multitasking. 相似文献
205.
Frederick M. J. Lichacz Ph.D. 《Military psychology》2013,25(3):412-426
In the present article the calibration of situation awareness and confidence within a multinational coalition was examined. The results revealed differential levels of situational awareness and calibration across the members of the coalition. These findings are discussed in terms of both linguistic and cultural differences within the coalition and the potential implications that poor calibration can have for decision-making and overall team performance. 相似文献
206.
预先承诺是指个体提前做出一个有长远目标的决定来限制对即时诱惑的选择,从而避免在未来决策中由于意志力缺乏而出现的自我控制失败。最后期限法和限制选择法是开展预先承诺研究的常用范式。本研究在系统回顾预先承诺的三种理论解释(自我控制资源模型、神经网络模型和时间差强化学习模型)的基础上,尝试提出一个整合性的认知神经机制模型来解释预先承诺的作用过程。未来研究可进一步丰富预先承诺的研究范式和应用范围,并探究预先承诺调控非理性决策的心理与神经机制。 相似文献
207.
208.
Three experiments examined the effects of constant vs. varied input of letter strings on recall, and then examined the effects of such training in the learning of new lists of letter strings. Letter strings were constructed from pairs of trigrams spatially grouped, and were presented either consistently or with different spatial groupings on successive presentations. In Experiments I and II, varied input produced substantially greater recall than constant input. When transferred to a new list of letter strings, containing either the same general structure or a new scrambled structure, recall of the second list was determined primarily by conditions of first-list input, and unaffected by second-list structure. Although the "variability effect" did not appear in the training phase of Experiment III, Varied input led subjects to regroup or integrate the letter sequences more frequently and produced similar transfer effects as in Experiments I and II. 相似文献
209.
Weanling and mature rats were presented with saccharin or saline solutions for 1 h on alternate days. Following exposure to saccharin, rats were injected with 0, 21, or 37 mg/kg of cyclophosphamide. Injections had no significant effect on saccharin preference in one-stimulus tests, but had a highly significant effect in two-stimulus tests. 相似文献
210.
The brightness of pairs of photic pulses, differing in pulse duration and pulse separation. was measured. The data were in general agreement with the neurophysiological model of Bishop and Clare. Pulse duration appeared to have little systematic influence on the shape of the curves. Time between onset of the first and second pulse appeared to be a prime factor determining the receptivity of the cortex to the second pulse. The two pulses were seen as single below about .03 sec separation. The second pulse was always judged brighter than the first. 相似文献