全文获取类型
收费全文 | 478篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
480篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 17篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有480条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Efforts to characterize the memory system that supports sentence comprehension have historically drawn extensively on short-term memory as a source of mechanisms that might apply to sentences. The focus of these efforts has changed significantly in the past decade. As a result of changes in models of short-term working memory (ST-WM) and developments in models of sentence comprehension, the effort to relate entire components of an ST-WM system, such as those in the model developed by Baddeley (Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 829–839, 2003) to sentence comprehension has largely been replaced by an effort to relate more specific mechanisms found in modern models of ST-WM to memory processes that support one aspect of sentence comprehension—the assignment of syntactic structure (parsing) and its use in determining sentence meaning (interpretation) during sentence comprehension. In this article, we present the historical background to recent studies of the memory mechanisms that support parsing and interpretation and review recent research into this relation. We argue that the results of this research do not converge on a set of mechanisms derived from ST-WM that apply to parsing and interpretation. We argue that the memory mechanisms supporting parsing and interpretation have features that characterize another memory system that has been postulated to account for skilled performance—long-term working memory. We propose a model of the relation of different aspects of parsing and interpretation to ST-WM and long-term working memory. 相似文献
92.
93.
94.
95.
96.
Psychopathy is often used to settle disputes about the nature of moral judgment. The “trolley problem” is a familiar scenario in which psychopathy is used as a test case. Where a convergence in response to the trolley problem is registered between psychopathic subjects and non-psychopathic (normal) subjects, it is assumed that this convergence indicates that the capacity for making moral judgments is unimpaired in psychopathy. This, in turn, is taken to have implications for the dispute between motivation internalists and motivation externalists, for instance. In what follows, we want to do two things: firstly, we set out to question the assumption that convergence is informative of the capacity for moral judgment in psychopathy. Next, we consider a distinct feature of psychopathy which we think provides strong grounds for holding that the capacity for moral judgment is seriously impaired in psychopathic subjects. The feature in question is the psychopathic subject’s inability to make sincere apologies. Our central claim will be this: convergence in response to trolley problems does not tell us very much about the psychopathic subject’s capacity to make moral judgments, but his inability to make sincere apologies does provide us with strong grounds for holding that this capacity is seriously impaired in psychopathy. 相似文献
97.
Elizabeth Kahn 《Journal of applied philosophy》2014,31(3):223-236
This article identifies ‘the tragedy of the commons’ as an essentially aggregative harm and considers what agents in such a scenario owe to one another. It proposes that the duty to take reasonable precautions requires that agents make efforts to establish collective solutions to any essentially aggregative harm to which they would otherwise contribute. Baylor Johnson has argued that the general obligation to promote the common good requires that agents make efforts to establish a collective agreement to avert a potential tragedy of the commons. This article agrees with Johnson's analysis but argues that there are also negative duties, which give commons users additional moral reason to take collective action to prevent the destruction of the utility of the commons. These duties link commons users to resolving this particular problem and provide grounds to demand that they make greater efforts to prevent the destruction of the commons compared to bystanders. The article suggests that fulfilling negative duties can require establishing collective institutions with the power to regulate human action and interaction. This analysis could be applied to a range of social problems which result from the aggregation of actions and practices. 相似文献
98.
Dream reports were collected from normal subjects in an effort to determine the degree to which dream reports can be used to identify individual dreamers. Judges were asked to group the reports by their authors. The judges scored the reports correctly at chance levels. This finding indicated that dreams may be at least as much like each other as they are the signature of individual dreamers. Our results suggest that dream reports cannot be used to identify the individuals who produced them when identifiers like names and gender of friends and family members are removed from the dream report. In addition to using dreams to learn about an individual, we must look at dreams as telling us about important common or generic aspects of human consciousness. 相似文献
99.
100.
S A Woodward M H McManis J Kagan P Deldin N Snidman M Lewis V Kahn 《Developmental psychology》2001,37(4):533-538
Brainstem auditory evoked responses (BAERs) were evaluated on 10-12-year-old children (N = 56) who had been classified as high or low reactive to unfamiliar stimuli at 4 months of age. BAER measurement was selected because high reactive infants tend to become inhibited or fearful young children, and adult introverts have a faster latency to wave V of the BAER than do extroverts. Children previously classified as high reactive at 4 months had larger wave V components than did low reactive children, a finding that possibly suggests greater excitability in projections to the inferior colliculus. The fact that a fundamental feature of brainstem activity differentiated preadolescent children belonging to two early temperamental groups supports the value of gathering physiological data in temperament research. 相似文献