全文获取类型
收费全文 | 55229篇 |
免费 | 1197篇 |
国内免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
56438篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 603篇 |
2019年 | 654篇 |
2018年 | 909篇 |
2017年 | 926篇 |
2016年 | 1015篇 |
2015年 | 765篇 |
2014年 | 857篇 |
2013年 | 3851篇 |
2012年 | 1633篇 |
2011年 | 1675篇 |
2010年 | 1039篇 |
2009年 | 980篇 |
2008年 | 1389篇 |
2007年 | 1368篇 |
2006年 | 1201篇 |
2005年 | 1100篇 |
2004年 | 1063篇 |
2003年 | 991篇 |
2002年 | 977篇 |
2001年 | 1846篇 |
2000年 | 1769篇 |
1999年 | 1264篇 |
1998年 | 559篇 |
1997年 | 501篇 |
1992年 | 1144篇 |
1991年 | 1064篇 |
1990年 | 1089篇 |
1989年 | 982篇 |
1988年 | 974篇 |
1987年 | 918篇 |
1986年 | 976篇 |
1985年 | 1040篇 |
1984年 | 789篇 |
1983年 | 719篇 |
1982年 | 485篇 |
1981年 | 473篇 |
1979年 | 844篇 |
1978年 | 564篇 |
1977年 | 528篇 |
1976年 | 524篇 |
1975年 | 743篇 |
1974年 | 864篇 |
1973年 | 884篇 |
1972年 | 792篇 |
1971年 | 723篇 |
1970年 | 692篇 |
1969年 | 718篇 |
1968年 | 904篇 |
1967年 | 794篇 |
1966年 | 732篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
S Martin K Housley H Mccoy P Greenhouse F Stigger M A Kenney S Shoffner V Fu M Korslund F G Ercanli-Huffman 《Perceptual and motor skills》1988,67(3):879-884
The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale was administered to 550 14- and 16-yr.-old (+/- 6 mo.) girls. Self-esteem scores were categorized by weight and weight by height. Scores on the Quetelet Index for obesity were correlated with self-esteem scores. Mean self-esteem of the low- and middle-weight by height group was significantly higher than the mean of the high-weight by height group. In analyzing weight alone, the self-esteem of the middle-weight group was significantly higher than the self-esteem of the high-weight group. The correlation of the obesity index and self-esteem indicated that as weight increased self-esteem decreased. 相似文献
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
LISE M. SAARI TERRY R. JOHNSON STEVEN D. MCLAUGHLIN DENISE M. ZIMMERLE 《Personnel Psychology》1988,41(4):731-743
A survey was systematically developed to assess issues related to management training and education, and it was sent to 1,000 randomly selected U.S. companies with at least 1,000 employees. Based on a 61% response rate, findings were obtained regarding issues such as needs assessment, management training and development approaches, reasons for selecting particular programs, characteristics of participants, how decisions are made regarding who will participate, preparation and follow-up of participants, evaluation of management training programs, future management training trends, and needed training content. Significant findings by company size and industry type also are reported. 相似文献
47.
For 3 consecutive days cockroaches (Periplaneta americana) were exposed to either escapable, inescapable, or no shock in an escape task. Twenty-four hours later they were tested in a shuttlebox escape task. There were reliable differences between escapable and inescapable animals and between inescapable and control animals in both escape latencies and the number of failures to escape. 相似文献
48.
M R Lamb 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1988,14(1):96-104
Pigeons in a matching-to-sample experiment received (a) element samples (one of two colors or vertical or horizontal white lines), (b) separated compound samples (a color and a line element presented together), and (c) unified compound samples (vertical or horizontal colored lines). In Phase I, test stimuli were the two elements from one dimension; the dimension tested varied randomly when the sample was a compound. Element samples were matched better than unified samples, which were matched better than separated samples. In Phase II, two samples preceded the tests. On EC trials, the first sample (S1) was an element, and the second sample (S2) was a compound; on CE trials this order was reversed. One element of the compound sample was always the one appearing as the element sample on that trial (e.g., S1 = red, S2 = red-vertical). This element also served as the correct test alternative (e.g., test = red+ vs. green) except on probe trials when the correct test was the other element appearing in the compound (e.g., test = vertical+ vs. horizontal). Accuracy varied as a function of sample sequence when the compound was separated (EC greater than CE on nonprobe trials and CE greater than EC on prove trials). This pattern was not observed when the compound was unified. The data suggest that the element in EC trials served as a cue that increased processing of the cued dimension and decreased processing of the noncued dimension of separated compounds. However, both dimensions of unified compounds seem to have been processed regardless of cuing.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS) 相似文献
49.
Fighting pairs of isolated DBA/2 mice showed a significant increase in tail-flick response latencies independent of whether opponents were losing or winning the combat. The effect lasted less than 10 min in both animals. Elevated pain thresholds were also found in isolates that attacked a nonaggressive conspecific, and were prevented by naltrexone (0.2 mg/kg), while a larger dose (1.0 mg/kg) inhibited the attack behavior. A small increase in pain threshold was observed after exposure of isolates to the test box alone, while isolation per se had no effect on baseline tail-flick latencies. The data demonstrate that endogenous pain suppressing systems are activated during attack and suggest that this opioid-mediated antinociception is a correlate of the isolation syndrome, reflecting enhanced arousal of the attacking animal. 相似文献
50.
M R D'Amato M Colombo 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1988,14(2):131-139
Cebus monkeys were trained on a five-item serial learning task, symbolized as ABCDE; the initial stages of training were on the shorter subseries AB, ABC, and ABCD. To assess the monkeys' knowledge of the sequential position of each item, pair-wise tests were given to 2 subjects after acquisition of the ABCD series and to 4 subjects after reaching criterion on the ABCDE series. In both tests, the monkeys performed at high levels on the interior pairs, which were BC for the ABCD series, and BC, BD, and CD for the ABCDE series. These results, as well as the orderly relations observed in the pair-wise tests between first-item response latency and first-item position and between second-item response latency and number of missing items, indicated that the monkeys had developed a well-organized internal representation of the four- and five-item series. Although pigeons are also capable of learning four-item and five-item series, they apparently do not develop a comparable representational structure. The disparity between the monkeys' and pigeons' representational competence for serial order is predictable from the difference in their capacities for associative transitivity. 相似文献