全文获取类型
收费全文 | 38137篇 |
免费 | 822篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
38970篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 475篇 |
2019年 | 477篇 |
2018年 | 744篇 |
2017年 | 697篇 |
2016年 | 731篇 |
2015年 | 517篇 |
2014年 | 655篇 |
2013年 | 2778篇 |
2012年 | 1157篇 |
2011年 | 1273篇 |
2010年 | 718篇 |
2009年 | 769篇 |
2008年 | 1037篇 |
2007年 | 1015篇 |
2006年 | 910篇 |
2005年 | 791篇 |
2004年 | 769篇 |
2003年 | 748篇 |
2002年 | 699篇 |
2001年 | 1403篇 |
2000年 | 1379篇 |
1999年 | 974篇 |
1998年 | 406篇 |
1997年 | 371篇 |
1993年 | 319篇 |
1992年 | 804篇 |
1991年 | 705篇 |
1990年 | 790篇 |
1989年 | 707篇 |
1988年 | 669篇 |
1987年 | 625篇 |
1986年 | 671篇 |
1985年 | 708篇 |
1984年 | 541篇 |
1983年 | 466篇 |
1982年 | 337篇 |
1979年 | 504篇 |
1978年 | 409篇 |
1977年 | 320篇 |
1976年 | 317篇 |
1975年 | 471篇 |
1974年 | 568篇 |
1973年 | 531篇 |
1972年 | 466篇 |
1971年 | 392篇 |
1970年 | 348篇 |
1969年 | 414篇 |
1968年 | 469篇 |
1967年 | 402篇 |
1966年 | 440篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
The role of the hippocampal system in retrograde and anterograde amnesia was investigated by using a novel olfactory-guided
paradigm and a traditional test of spatial learning. In the retrograde study, rats were trained on a sequence of two-choice
olfactory discriminations in the weeks prior to receiving neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus or aspiration lesions of the
perirhinal-entorhinal cortex. Memory tests for preoperatively learned discriminations revealed no statistical impairment for
subjects with damage to the hippocampus on a problem learned remote in time from surgery (i.e., 4 weeks +) or on the two recently
learned discriminations (i.e., 1–3 weeks prior to surgery). The performance of subjects with perirhinal-entorhinal damage
provided an important comparison for subjects with specific hippocampal lesions. Despite showing intact memory for the remotely
learned problem, perirhinalentorhinal damage resulted in numerically (although not significantly) weaker performance on postoperative
tests of retention for the discriminations learned in the 3 weeks prior to surgery. In the anterograde portion of the study,
long-term memory for newly acquired discriminations was spared in subjects with damage to the hippocampus, whereas subjects
in the perirhinal-entorhinal lesion group again showed the weakest memory performance on these tests of 5-day retention. Postoperative
water maze learning was uniformly impaired in subjects with damage to the hippocampus and perirhinalentorhinal cortex, thus
confirming the effect of these lesions and supporting the involvement of these brain areas in spatial processes. These findings
further dissociate the specific involvement of the hippocampus in tasks of a spatial-relational nature versus nonrelational
tasks, such as discrimination learning and recognition memory (e.g., Duva et al., 1997; Eichenbaum, 1997; Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum,
Young, & Bunsey, 1996). Moreover, the results suggest that damage to the hippocampus itself does not contribute to retrograde
or anterograde memory impairments for all types of information, whereas the data suggest a more important role for the perirhinal-entorhinal cortex in recognition
memory, irrespective of modality. 相似文献
992.
Jeff P. Hamm 《Behavior research methods》2001,33(4):532-539
Object-oriented programming provides a useful structure for designing reusable code. Accurate millisecond timing is essential for many areas of research. With this in mind, this paper provides a Turbo Pascal unit containing an object-oriented millisecond timer. This approach allows for multiple timers to be running independently. The timers may also be set at different levels of temporal precision, such as 10−3 (milliseconds) or 10−5 sec. The object also is able to store the time of a flagged event for later examination without interrupting the ongoing timing operation. 相似文献
993.
994.
The finding of stronger affective priming in less conscious (suboptimal) conditions than in fully conscious (optimal) conditions (S. T. Murphy & R. B. Zajonc, 1993) is theoretically important because it contradicts notions that emotions are primarily reflected by conscious states. In 2 experiments, this pattern of results was obtained. Happy and angry faces were presented both optimally and suboptimally and were masked by unknown ideographs. In Experiment 1, instructions for the conscious and less conscious affective priming conditions were matched, and affective ratings of ideographs were determined. In Experiment 2, a more implicit affective measure (facial electromyography of musculus zygomaticus major and musculus corrugator supercilii) served as the dependent variable. Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming was found in both experiments. It is concluded that stronger suboptimal than optimal processing is characteristic for affective processing and that it can also be found when instructions are matched and when a more implicit measure is assessed. 相似文献
995.
Jones RT Frary R Cunningham P Weddle JD Kaiser L 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2001,7(1):103-108
The impact of Hurricane Andrew on 212 African American, Caucasian, and Hispanic elementary and middle school children was examined at 6 months postdisaster. Using self-report instruments, this case study examined the predictive utility of several hypothesized mediators of children's reactions to disaster. Results showed higher levels of intrusive symptomatology for girls and for elementary school children as compared with their middle school counterparts. No differences were found with reference to race. The lack of findings concerning race is addressed, as well as implications for future studies. 相似文献
996.
997.
998.
Previous research has shown that a considerable number of primates can remember the location and fruiting state of individual
trees in their home range. This enables them to relocate fruit or predict whether previously encountered fruit has ripened.
Recent studies, however, suggest that the ability of primates to cognitively map fruit-bearing trees is limited. In this study,
we investigated an alternative and arguably simpler, more efficient strategy, the use of synchrony, a botanical characteristic
of a large number of fruit species. Synchronous fruiting would allow the prediction of the fruiting state of a large number
of trees without having to first check the trees. We studied whether rainforest primates, grey-cheeked mangabeys in the Kibale
National Park, Uganda, used synchrony in fruit emergence to find fruit. We analysed the movements of adult males towards Uvariopsis congensis food trees, a strongly synchronous fruiting species with different local patterns of synchrony. Monkeys approached within
crown distance, entered and inspected significantly more Uvariopsis trees when the percentage of trees with ripe fruit was high compared to when it was low. Since the effect was also found
for empty trees, the monkeys likely followed a synchrony-based inspection strategy. We found no indication that the monkeys
generalised this strategy to all Uvariopsis trees within their home range. Instead, they attended to fruiting peaks in local areas within the home range and adjusted
their inspective behaviour accordingly revealing that non-human primates use botanical knowledge in a flexible way. 相似文献
999.
Vaishali V. Raval Pratiksha H. Raval Stephen P. Becker 《Journal of child and family studies》2012,21(2):320-330
Cultural influences in the experience and expression of emotions have been widely recognized, although systematic studies
of emotion-related beliefs in culturally diverse families have been scarce. The purpose of the present exploratory study was
to examine beliefs about anger among a group of adolescent male offenders in India temporarily residing in a pre-sentencing
institution. In particular, adolescent offenders (n = 14) were individually interviewed about their beliefs concerning causes and concomitants of, and behavioral responses to,
one’s own and others’ anger, as well as family patterns of anger expression that are crucial in understanding socialization
processes. Although some of the themes reflected in their narratives parallel the beliefs of educated, middle-class, non-offender
youth in India (e.g., social inappropriateness of anger), as well as those of offenders in other parts of the world (e.g.,
aggressive behavior as a common response to anger), beliefs about various aspects of anger specific to this subculture of
offender youth also emerged. In particular, perceived violations of family honor were cited as the most common elicitors of
anger, and although anger was considered ‘bad’ it was expressed through aggression. The findings of this study highlight the
need to develop a culturally informed theory of emotions, and relatedly, to develop culturally sensitive interventions for
anger management that can be incorporated as a part of rehabilitation efforts for offender youth in India. 相似文献
1000.
Mental rotation is thought to underlie the increase in response times (RTs) for deciding whether rotated letters are normal
or mirrored versions. However, mental rotation predicts a linear increase in RTs, whereas the mirror/normal letter discrimination
task typically produces a curved function. Recently, Kung and Hamm suggested that this curved function results from a mixture
of trials in which mental rotation is employed and trials in which it is not. The mixture ratio may vary between individuals,
with some individuals relying more on mental rotation than others. There is no factor in the Kung and Hamm model that reflects
such individual differences. In the present study, we suggest that a possible exponent parameter could be added to the Kung
and Hamm model to capture individual differences in the mixture ratio. This exponent parameter appears to capture an individual
characteristic since the value obtained correlates between the mirror/normal letter task and a left/right object facing task.
The development of a quantity that represents the mixture ratio will aid further testing of processes involved in the visual
imagery system. 相似文献