首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   75823篇
  免费   3003篇
  国内免费   33篇
  2020年   733篇
  2019年   937篇
  2018年   1375篇
  2017年   1383篇
  2016年   1527篇
  2015年   1092篇
  2014年   1210篇
  2013年   5450篇
  2012年   2399篇
  2011年   2617篇
  2010年   1496篇
  2009年   1465篇
  2008年   2096篇
  2007年   2086篇
  2006年   1904篇
  2005年   2756篇
  2004年   2124篇
  2003年   1814篇
  2002年   1658篇
  2001年   2440篇
  2000年   2413篇
  1999年   1780篇
  1998年   821篇
  1997年   743篇
  1996年   717篇
  1993年   646篇
  1992年   1504篇
  1991年   1377篇
  1990年   1407篇
  1989年   1278篇
  1988年   1252篇
  1987年   1191篇
  1986年   1273篇
  1985年   1273篇
  1984年   1063篇
  1983年   954篇
  1982年   658篇
  1981年   657篇
  1979年   1089篇
  1978年   791篇
  1975年   917篇
  1974年   1028篇
  1973年   1083篇
  1972年   940篇
  1971年   858篇
  1970年   762篇
  1969年   833篇
  1968年   1030篇
  1967年   908篇
  1966年   807篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
21.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
22.
This article integrates existing theoretical perspectives on message content and negotiator motivation to formulate a comprehensive definitional model of the interrelationships among communication behaviors in crisis negotiation. A sample of 189 nuclear dialogue spans were transcribed from 9 resolved cases of hostage negotiation and each utterance coded at the level of thought units across 41 behavioral variables. Results of a nonmetric, multidimensional scaling solution provided clear support for the hypothesized cylindrical structure of communication behavior, revealing 3 dominant levels of suspect‐negotiator interaction (Avoidance, Distributive, Integrative). At each level of the structure, interactions were found to modulate around 3 thematic styles of communication (Identity, Instrumental, Relational), which reflected the underlying motivational emphasis of individuals' dialogue. Finally, the intensity of communication was found to play a polarizing role in the cylinder, with intense, functionally discrete behaviors occurring toward the boundary of the structure.  相似文献   
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
Deflationism and Tarski's paradise   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Ketland  J 《Mind》1999,108(429):69-94
  相似文献   
28.
29.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
30.
When some pictures are mirror reversed, aesthetic evaluations of them change dramatically. Stimulus features that may be important in contributing to this effect are: (a) location of areas of principal interest or weight in the picture space, (b) cues that suggest a direction of motion within the picture. Dextrals and inverted sinistrals preferred paintings with cues suggesting motion proceeding from left to right over their mirror-reversed versions and also preferred those with weight concentrated in the left portions of the picture space. The explanation that best fits these data is that preference is promoted when the picture content encourages attention to its rightmost portions, thus placing a majority of the picture in the left visual field where it is directly processed by the right hemisphere.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号