首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   47570篇
  免费   2022篇
  国内免费   22篇
  49614篇
  2020年   511篇
  2019年   642篇
  2018年   879篇
  2017年   901篇
  2016年   915篇
  2015年   672篇
  2014年   804篇
  2013年   3582篇
  2012年   1490篇
  2011年   1628篇
  2010年   960篇
  2009年   919篇
  2008年   1324篇
  2007年   1270篇
  2006年   1188篇
  2005年   1050篇
  2004年   1025篇
  2003年   941篇
  2002年   980篇
  2001年   1604篇
  2000年   1504篇
  1999年   1096篇
  1998年   542篇
  1997年   464篇
  1996年   431篇
  1992年   863篇
  1991年   811篇
  1990年   828篇
  1989年   822篇
  1988年   799篇
  1987年   738篇
  1986年   792篇
  1985年   832篇
  1984年   655篇
  1983年   607篇
  1982年   462篇
  1981年   453篇
  1979年   683篇
  1978年   520篇
  1976年   471篇
  1975年   594篇
  1974年   697篇
  1973年   693篇
  1972年   579篇
  1971年   525篇
  1970年   511篇
  1969年   569篇
  1968年   654篇
  1967年   563篇
  1966年   573篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
Lesions of the hypothalamic ventromedial nucleus (VMN) led to an immediate increase in food intake but not to any increase in hoarding activity. Later, when subjects were very obese, they failed to hoard even in response to a 16-hr. deprivation schedule, although this schedule did produce hoarding if body weights were held at, or were brought down to, preoperative levels. These results indicate that the lateral hypothalamic mechanism responsible for hoarding and feeding responds to long-term nutritional factors, and that it is not directly affected by the short-term satiety mechanisms in the VMN. These findings support the hypothesis (a) that even “non-physiological” activities (e.g. hoarding, exploration) are motivated by physiological needs, and (b) that the reason these activities do not ordinarily covary with physiological drives is that, unlike the physiological drives, they are not subject to inhibition by the hypothalamic satiety mechanisms.  相似文献   
952.
A unimanual, two-digit choice-reaction-time task was used in conjunction with visual signals presented in the extreme peripheral field. The position of the signal on the retina, the eye stimulated, the hand used in responding, and the operating digits were all controlled for their effects. Stimulus-response compatibility was maintained throughout. It was found that RT to ipsilateral S-R pathways was consistently faster than when the pathways involved transcommissural connections. Subsidiary findings related to differences between the operating digits and the side of signal occurrence. These effects are discussed in connection with known interhemispheric phenomena and other CRT findings.  相似文献   
953.
954.
The plastic barrel suit provides complete protection of chronically implanted fistulas and electrical connectors. The lost time, expense, and the frustrations of having these devices damaged was avoided.  相似文献   
955.
956.
Ten Ss served in this experiment, five right-handed and five left-handed. Every S was tested in a perceived-order situation and by the up-and-down method to determine the relative on-latency for a visual test stimulus. i.e, (ON-Lat for lest stimulus involving right hemisphere- ON-Lat for standard stimulus involving left hemisphere) and to determine a similarly defined relative off-latency for the same test stimulus. The algebraic difference between the relative on-latency measure and the relative off-latency measure was then found. Data from a previous study had suggested that this “on-off difference” was characteristically positive for left-handed Ss and negative for right-handed Ss. The present data agree. The left-handed Ss were found to differ significantly from the right-handed Ss in the magnitude of the on-off difference. This outcome appears important as a possible clue to functional interhemispheric differences related to handedness.  相似文献   
957.
Two-key concurrent responding was maintained for three pigeons by a single variable-interval 1-minute schedule of reinforcement in conjunction with a random number generator that assigned feeder operations between keys with equal probability. The duration of blackouts was varied between keys when each response initiated a blackout, and grain arranged by the variable-interval schedule was automatically presented after a blackout (Exp. I). In Exp. II every key peck, except for those that produced grain, initiated a blackout, and grain was dependent upon a response following a blackout. For each pigeon in Exp. I and for one pigeon in Exp. II, the relative frequency of responding on a key approximated, i.e., matched, the relative reciprocal of the duration of the blackout interval on that key. In a third experiment, blackouts scheduled on a variable-interval were of equal duration on the two keys. For one key, grain automatically followed each blackout; for the other key, grain was dependent upon a response and never followed a blackout. The relative frequency of responding on the former key, i.e., the delay key, better approximated the negative exponential function obtained by Chung (1965) than the matching function predicted by Chung and Herrnstein (1967).  相似文献   
958.
Choice for periodic schedules of reinforcement   总被引:17,自引:17,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Pigeons' responses in the presence of two concurrently available (initial-link) stimuli produced one of two different (terminal-link) stimuli according to identical but independent variable-interval schedules. Responding in the mutually exclusive terminal links was reinforced with food according to fixed-ratio schedules for six pigeons and according to fixed-interval schedules for two pigeons. None of the pigeons matched the proportion of (choice) responses in the initial links to the proportion of the rates of reinforcement obtained during the terminal links. Instead, as the values of each of the terminal-link schedules were increased by a constant amount, the choice proportions for the stimulus associated with the smaller of the two values increased, even though the relative rates of reinforcement during the terminal links decreased. These results are incompatible with those from previous studies with aperiodic (variable-interval or variable-ratio) schedules. The present results do suggest, however, that in transforming aperiodic schedules into their periodic equivalents, it may be necessary to consider the size of the smallest interreinforcement interval comprising the terminal-link schedules.  相似文献   
959.
Some punishing effects of response-force   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
The present experiment explored the punishing effect of different response-force requirements by means of a two-operant design analogous to a two-component chain schedule. The first component of the chain required a lever pull through 0.25 in. (0.64 cm) at 1 lb (4.45 N) of force. The second component required a lever pull through an additional 0.75 in. (1.90 cm) with the force varied between sessions from 1 lb to 50 lb (4.45 N to 223 N). Completion of the second component of the chain was reinforced after variable intervals averaging 1 min. The average rate of first-component response decreased as the force requirement for second-component responses was increased. This rate reduction did not appear to be due to increased response duration, “fatigue”, or differing rates of reinforcement. If the force requirement for the second-component response is viewed as a consequence for the first-component response, then the results of the experiment show that a high force requirement is a punisher.  相似文献   
960.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号