全文获取类型
收费全文 | 767篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
778篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 37篇 |
2013年 | 83篇 |
2012年 | 55篇 |
2011年 | 59篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 24篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有778条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
242.
David Trafimow Justin A. MacDonald Stephen Rice 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2012,74(6):1355-1365
According to many theories of decision making, of which signal detection theory is the most prominent, randomness is the main factor responsible for imperfect performance. These theories imply that correcting for attenuation due to randomness should result in perfect scores as long as the participants use nonextreme decision criteria. On the basis of a recent advance termed potential performance theory (Trafimow & Rice, Psychological Review 115:447-462, 2008), we performed auditory and visual detection experiments and corrected the scores for attenuation. Most participants in both experiments tended to perform at a less-than-perfect level, even after their scores were corrected. The findings demonstrate that at least one systematic factor influences detection that is not included in signal detection theory. 相似文献
243.
Bertram J. Cohler Justin L. Weiss Henry U. Grunebaum 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(6):563-572
Using an abbreviated version of content scales derived by a rational-theoretical approach to the MMPI item pool rather than the traditional criterion group approach, this report demonstrates both satisfactory long- short-form correlations and promising internal consistency for these short-form scales. Comparing women discharged from a psychiatric hospital (N = 40) and non-psychiatric controls (N = 41), these content scales differentiate the two groups both at the beginning of an aftercare intervention study and approximately 20 months later. Mood scales are somewhat less stable over time, especially among patients. While not correlated with age or verbal intelligence, several scales are correlated with measures of social status. 相似文献
244.
The standard view in epistemology is that propositional knowledge entails belief. Positive arguments are seldom given for this entailment thesis, however; instead, its truth is typically assumed. Against the entailment thesis, Myers-Schulz and Schwitzgebel (Noûs, forthcoming) report that a non-trivial percentage of people think that there can be propositional knowledge without belief. In this paper, we add further fuel to the fire, presenting the results of four new studies. Based on our results, we argue that the entailment thesis does not deserve the default status that it is typically granted. We conclude by considering the alternative account of knowledge that Myers-Schulz and Schwitzgebel propose to explain their results, arguing that it does not explain ours. In its place we offer a different explanation of both sets of findings—the conviction account, according to which belief, but not knowledge, requires mental assent. 相似文献
245.
The Psychological Record - In two experiments, we examined the disruptive effects of a “can’t answer” response option (CARO) on equivalence formation. The first experiment was a... 相似文献
246.
Machery, Mallon, Nichols, and Stich [2004; forthcoming] use experimental methods to raise a spectre of doubt about reliance on intuitions in developing theories of reference which are then deployed in philosophical arguments outside the philosophy of language. Machery et al. ran a cross-cultural survey asking Western and East Asian participants about a famous case from the philosophical literature on reference (Kripke's Gödel example). They interpret their results as indicating that there is significant variation in participants' intuitions about semantic reference for that case. We argue that this interpretation is mistaken. We detail a type of ambiguity found in Machery et al.'s probe but not yet noted in the response literature. We argue that this epistemic ambiguity could have affected their results. We do not stop there, however: Rather than rest content with a possibility claim, we ran four studies to test the impact of this ambiguity on participants' responses. We found that this accounts for much of the variation in Machery et al.'s original experiment. We conclude that in the light of our new data, their argument is no longer convincing. 相似文献
247.
248.
When other individuals move, we interpret their movements as discrete, hierarchically-organized, goal-directed actions. However, the mechanisms that integrate visible movement features into actions are poorly understood. Here, we consider two sequence learning mechanisms – transitional probability-based (TP) and position-based encoding computations – that have been studied extensively in the domain of language learning, and investigate their potential for integrating movements into actions. If these learning mechanisms integrate movements into actions, then they should create memory units that contain (i) movement information, (ii) information about the order in which movements occurred, and (iii) information allowing actions to be recognized from different viewpoints. We show that both mechanisms retain movement information. However, only the position-based mechanism creates movement representations that are view-invariant and contain order information. The TP-based mechanism creates movement representations that are view-dependent and contain no order information. We therefore suggest that the TP-based mechanism is unlikely to play an important role for integrating movements into actions. In contrast, the position-based mechanism retains some of the types of information needed to represent goal-directed actions, which makes it an attractive target for further research to explore what, if any, role it plays in the perception of goal-directed actions. 相似文献
249.
Deborah J. Brief Amy Rubin Justin L. Enggasser Monica Roy Terence M. Keane 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2011,41(4):237-246
A substantial number of military personnel who have served in Iraq (Operation Iraqi Freedom; OIF) and Afghanistan (Operating
Enduring Freedom; OEF) develop symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in response to their military experiences
and many of these same individuals will drink in a risky or problematic manner following deployment. If left untreated, PTSD
symptoms and alcohol problems can become chronic and have a significant, negative impact on the lives of veterans, their families
and communities. Further, OIF and OEF service members are often reluctant to seek treatment for mental health symptoms or
alcohol problems secondary to stigma. In order to reach this population it is essential that new strategies and venues for
delivering evidence-based care are explored. Web-based interventions are uniquely suited to this cohort of veterans in that
they have the potential to reach a significant number of veterans who commonly use the Web and who might not otherwise receive
care. This article will review the prevalence of PTSD and alcohol problems among OIF and OEF veterans, common barriers they
experience with accessing care in traditional mental health settings, and what is known about the effectiveness of Web-based
approaches for PTSD and alcohol problems. It also describes the components of a new Web-based intervention, developed by the
authors, that uses motivational enhancement and cognitive-behavioral strategies to intervene with returning veterans who report
PTSD symptoms and problem drinking. Recommendations for future directions in working with returning veterans with PTSD and
alcohol problems will be offered. 相似文献
250.
Cross Racial Identity Scale (CRIS) scores: stability and relationships with psychological adjustment
Worrell FC Mendoza-Denton R Telesford J Simmons C Martin JF 《Journal of personality assessment》2011,93(6):637-648
We examined the structural validity, internal consistency (alpha and omega), and test-retest reliability of scores on the Cross Racial Identity Scale (CRIS; Vandiver et al., 2000 ; Worrell, Vandiver, & Cross, 2004 ), as well as the relationship between CRIS scores and several variables related to psychological adjustment. Participants consisted of several groups of African American college students (34 ≤ n ≤ 340) attending a predominantly White university in a Western state. Confirmatory factor analyses indicated an acceptable fit of the data to the theoretical model, and alpha and omega coefficients indicate that CRIS scores have moderate to high internal consistency. CRIS scores also demonstrated stability over periods between 2 and 20 months in ranges that suggest long-term stability of racial attitudes. As predicted by the expanded nigrescence model (Cross & Vandiver, 2001 ), only self-hatred attitudes had consistent, meaningful relationships with psychological adjustment. 相似文献