全文获取类型
收费全文 | 775篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
786篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 39篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 37篇 |
2013年 | 84篇 |
2012年 | 55篇 |
2011年 | 59篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 24篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有786条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
162.
Annie Roy-Charland Melanie Perron Cheryl Young Jessica Boulard Justin A. Chamberland 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(5):281-298
The goal of the present study was to test the Perceptual-Attentional Limitation Hypothesis in children and adults by manipulating the distinctiveness between expressions and recording eye movements. Children 3–5 and 9–11 years old as well as adults were presented pairs of expressions and required to identify a target emotion. Children 3–5 years old were less accurate than those 9–11 years old and adults. All children viewed pictures longer than adults but did not spend more time attending to the relevant cues. For all participants, accuracy for the recognition of fear was lower than for surprise when the distinctive cue was in the brow only. They also took longer and spent more time in both the mouth and brow zones than when a cue was in the mouth or both areas. Adults and children 9–11 years old made more comparisons between the expressions when fear comprised a single distinctive cue in the brow than when the distinctive cue was in the mouth only or when both cues were present. Children 3–5 years old made more comparisons for brow only than both. The results of the present study extend on the Perceptual-Attentional Limitation Hypothesis showing an importance of both decoder and stimuli, and an interaction between decoder and stimuli characteristics. 相似文献
163.
Justin D. Caouette Sarah K. Ruiz Clinton C. Lee Zainab Anbari Roberta A. Schriber Amanda E. Guyer 《Cognition & emotion》2013,27(5):945-953
Social anxiety (SA) involves a multitude of cognitive symptoms related to fear of evaluation, including expectancy and memory biases. We examined whether memory biases are influenced by expectancy biases for social feedback in SA. We hypothesised that, faced with a socially evaluative event, people with higher SA would show a negative expectancy bias for future feedback. Furthermore, we predicted that memory bias for feedback in SA would be mediated by expectancy bias. Ninety-four undergraduate students (55 women, mean age = 19.76 years) underwent a two-visit task that measured expectations about (Visit 1) and memory of (Visit 2) feedback from unknown peers. Results showed that higher levels of SA were associated with negative expectancy bias. An indirect relationship was found between SA and memory bias that was mediated by expectancy bias. The results suggest that expectancy biases are in the causal path from SA to negative memory biases for social evaluation. 相似文献
164.
Meagan C. Alder W. Justin Dyer Jonathan G. Sandberg Stephanie Y. Davis Julianne Holt-Lunstad 《The American journal of family therapy》2013,41(5):541-555
AbstractDecades of research have shown that depression can have deleterious effects on couple relationships. Emotionally-focused therapy (EFT; Johnson, 2004) is an empirically validated form of couple therapy, which has shown effectiveness in reducing depressive symptoms in couples where the wife has depression. The aim of the current pilot study was to examine the extent to which EFT and the comparison treatment as usual (TAU) therapy group decrease depressive symptoms in distressed couples. Results show that both groups were effective in decreasing depressive symptoms, with EFT showing a clinically significant reduction. Clinical implications and future research are discussed. 相似文献
165.
Justin Zylstra 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2013,56(8):859-876
In this paper, I develop a truthmaker semantics for essence and use the semantics to investigate the explanatory role of essence. 相似文献
166.
AbstractFeminist therapy altered how marriage and family therapists conceptualized their clients’ realities and psychopathology. Upon an extensive search of the literature, there is a sparse amount of literature regarding the integration of feminist therapy with experiential therapy. The authors discuss feminist and experiential therapy models, propose a model of feminist-experiential therapy, and highlight the need and fit of these theories. A discussion on giving space to silenced voices, establishing an egalitarian relationship, expanding the definition of deviance, and exploring the importance of language will be done to aid the experiential therapist in their work and expand their client’s experience. 相似文献
167.
Justin B. Leaf Kathleen H. Tsuji Amy E. Lentell Stephanie E. Dale Alyne Kassardjian Mitchell Taubman John McEachin Ronald Leaf Misty L. Oppenheim‐Leaf 《Behavioral Interventions》2013,28(1):82-106
Discrete trial teaching (DTT) is a systematic form of intervention commonly implemented with children and adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Experimenters and clinicians have implemented DTT in both one‐to‐one instructional formats and group instructional formats to teach a wide variety of skills to children and adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. The purpose of this study was to compare DTT implemented in a one‐to‐one instructional format with DTT implemented in a group instructional format in order to determine which format was more effective, efficient, resulted in higher observational learning, and resulted in better maintenance when teaching a variety of expressive skills. The experimenters utilized a parallel treatment design, and the results indicated that both instructional formats were equally effective, there were mixed results in terms of maintenance and efficiency, and group instruction resulted in observational learning. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
168.
169.
In visual search, observers make decisions about the presence or absence of a target based on their perception of a target during search. The present study investigated whether decisions can be based on observers’ expectation rather than perception of a target. In Experiment 1, participants were allowed to make target-present responses by clicking on the target or, if the target was not perceived, a target-present button. Participants used the target-present button option more frequently in difficult search trials and when target prevalence was high. Experiment 2 and 3 employed a difficult search task that encouraged the use of prevalence-based decisions. Target presence was reported faster when target prevalence was high, indicating that decisions were, in part, cognitive, and not strictly perceptual. A similar pattern of responses were made even when no targets appeared in the search (Experiment 3). The implication of these prevalence-based decisions for visual search models is discussed. 相似文献
170.
Melissa E. Libertus Darko Odic Lisa Feigenson Justin Halberda 《Journal of cognition and development》2013,14(3):442-454
Measuring individual differences in children's emerging language abilities is important to researchers and clinicians alike. The 2 most widely used methods for assessing children's vocabulary both have limitations: Experimenter-administered tests are time-consuming and expensive, and parent questionnaires have only been designed for children up to 37 months of age. Here, we test the validity of a new assessment to fill this gap: the Developmental Vocabulary Assessment for Parents (DVAP). In 4 experiments, we assess the reliability of this measure and its concurrent and predictive validity in samples of 2- to 7-year-old children. We found that the DVAP provides a rapid, cost-effective alternative to experimenter-administered vocabulary tests for children. 相似文献