全文获取类型
收费全文 | 707篇 |
免费 | 61篇 |
专业分类
768篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 37篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 36篇 |
2013年 | 82篇 |
2012年 | 56篇 |
2011年 | 58篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 24篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有768条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
41.
Samuel W. Kent Andrew D. Wilson Mandy S. Plumb Justin H. G. Williams Mark Mon-Williams 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(1):10-15
The authors review studies of mentally simulated movements. In automatic or cyclical movements, actual and motor imagery (MI) durations are similar. When athletes simulate only dynamic phases of movement or perform MI just before competing, however, environmental and time constraints lead to an underestimation of actual duration. Conversely, complex attention-demanding movements take longer to image. Finally, participants can modify the speed of MI voluntarily when they receive specific instructions. To complete the available data, the authors compared imagined and actual durations in tennis and gymnastics. Results showed systematic and disproportionate overestimation of actual duration. The authors found a relationship between complex motor skills and MI duration. They discuss the factors leading to over- and underestimation and the hypotheses that could be tested. 相似文献
42.
43.
Justin Miller 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》1983,19(2):153-172
Three distinct constructions of transference which have related though differentiable histories can be identified in Freud's writings. The postulation of discrete meta-psychological, clinical, and universal constructions was completed by 1915, although many significant revisions within those constructions were made thereafter. Most significant is the distinction drawn between the clinical and universal constructions which (while both are founded on the metapsychological construction) facilitates recognition of cultural critical uses of transference as distinct from the clinical transference phenomena and as applicable far beyond the confines of psychopathology. 相似文献
44.
45.
The Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (BFNE; M. R. Leary, 1983a) is often used to assess fear of negative evaluation, the core feature of social anxiety disorder. However, few studies have examined its psychometric properties in large samples of socially anxious patients. Although the BFNE yields a single total score, confirmatory factor analysis indicated a 2-factor solution to be more appropriate, with the 1st factor consisting of all straightforwardly worded items (BFNE-S) and the 2nd of all reverse-scored items (BFNE-R). Support was obtained for the convergent and discriminant validity of the BFNE and BFNE-S, but not the BFNE-R. These results suggest that standard scoring of the BFNE may not be optimal for patients with social anxiety disorder. 相似文献
46.
This paper describes a long-term follow-up of patients with panic disorder who received cognitive behaviour therapy within a randomized controlled trial. Of 89 patients eligible for follow-up, 28 (31.5%) were reassessed 6-8 years after commencement of treatment in the trial. No differences were found between those who were followed up and those lost to follow-up on most baseline measures including measures of panic-related psychopathology, or depression. Outcomes at long-term follow-up were significantly better than baseline measures of panic, avoidance and depression. In this sub-sample the effect of cognitive behaviour therapy for panic disorder appears to maintain over the long-term. 相似文献
47.
Are abstract representations of number – representations that are independent of the particular type of entities that are enumerated – a product of human language or culture, or do they trace back to human infancy? To address this question, four experiments investigated whether human infants discriminate between sequences of actions (jumps of a puppet) on the basis of numerosity. At 6 months, infants successfully discriminated four‐ versus eight‐jump sequences, when the continuous variables of sequence duration, jump duration, jump rate, jump interval and duration, and extent of motion were controlled, and rhythm was eliminated. In contrast, infants failed to discriminate two‐ versus four‐jump sequences, suggesting that infants fail to form cardinal number representations of small numbers of actions. Infants also failed to discriminate between sequences of four versus six jumps at 6 months, and succeeded at 9 months, suggesting that infants’ number representations are imprecise and increase in precision with age. All of these findings agree with those of studies using visual–spatial arrays and auditory sequences, providing evidence that a single, abstract system of number representation is present and functional in infancy. 相似文献
48.
Public attitudes toward abortion have long been an issue in American political debates. Theoretical understanding of influences
on abortion attitudes may assist researchers in determining contributors of the attribution. Accordingly, this study administered
a 40-item abortion opinion survey to 396 college students at a Midwestern university to determine potential factors correlated
with abortion attitudes. Several factors such as religious involvement, knowledge of someone who has an abortion, and one’s
definition as to when life begins were correlated with abortion attitudes. Furthermore, Democrats reported stronger pro-choice
views than Republicans did. Similarly, Liberals were more pro-choice oriented than Conservatives. Although causal relationships
were not directly explored, theoretical explanations and support provide for a thorough understanding of potential factors
of abortion attitude formation and a preliminary model. Future implications are also discussed. 相似文献
49.
50.
Melanie Perron Annie Roy-Charland Justin A. Chamberland Carolyn Bleach Annalie Pelot 《Motivation and emotion》2016,40(3):478-488
While a smile can reflect felt happiness, it can also be voluntarily produced, for instance, to mask negative emotions. Masking strategies are not always perfect and traces of the negative emotion can leak. The current study examined the role of traces of anger, sadness, fear and disgust in the judgment of authenticity of smiles. Participants judged the authenticity of the smiles while their eye movements were recorded. They were also asked if the stimuli comprised another emotion and, if so, what the emotion was. Results revealed that participants were sensitive to traces of negative emotions. Variations were observed between emotions with performance being best for traces of fear and lowest for traces of anger in the eyebrows in the judgment task. However, when the presence of a negative emotion was reported, participants were less accurate in identifying fear but more accurate in identifying anger. Furthermore, variations were observed as a function of the location of the trace whether in the mouth or eyes as a function of the emotion. Traces in the eyebrows were associated with better performance than traces in the mouth for sadness but the opposite was observed for anger. The performance at the judgment task was not linked to eye movement measures or explicit knowledge of the masked emotion. Future research should explore other explanation for the variations in performance in the judgments of authenticity of masking smiles such as emotional contagion. 相似文献