全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3668篇 |
免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
3706篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 32篇 |
2023年 | 22篇 |
2022年 | 37篇 |
2021年 | 47篇 |
2020年 | 71篇 |
2019年 | 84篇 |
2018年 | 106篇 |
2017年 | 134篇 |
2016年 | 137篇 |
2015年 | 110篇 |
2014年 | 123篇 |
2013年 | 446篇 |
2012年 | 188篇 |
2011年 | 191篇 |
2010年 | 121篇 |
2009年 | 120篇 |
2008年 | 173篇 |
2007年 | 143篇 |
2006年 | 147篇 |
2005年 | 137篇 |
2004年 | 131篇 |
2003年 | 116篇 |
2002年 | 107篇 |
2001年 | 51篇 |
2000年 | 55篇 |
1999年 | 51篇 |
1998年 | 58篇 |
1997年 | 47篇 |
1996年 | 41篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 29篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 27篇 |
1981年 | 31篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有3706条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Merith Anne Cosden Henry C. Ellis Dennis M. Feeney 《Journal of research in personality》1979,13(4):386-395
This experiment examined the role of cognitive flexibility-rigidity in memory. On the basis of several personality tests, subjects were classified as flexible, intermediate, or rigid, using a postexperimental blocking procedure. All subjects were given a perceptual grouping task, which masks the meaningful structure of the material. Subjects have the option of discovering the more meaningful structure consisting of word pairs, or alternatively, processing less meaningful chunks of letters. Results were that flexible subjects recalled substantially more items than intermediate and rigid subjects, in that order. In addition, subjects given different (varied) groupings of the letter chunks recalled substantially more than subjects given repeated presentations of the same grouping structure. Transfer tests indicated that subjects acquired different coding strategies under varied and constant input. Repetitious input led subjects to adopt inefficient strategies in processing the material to be remembered, whereas varied input led subjects to adopt efficient encoding strategies. 相似文献
32.
Rape—Relationships and Recovery is a group counseling model designed to intercede when crisis-oriented rape counseling ends. Major aspects of the model include (a) giving factual information and dispelling common myths surrounding rape; (b) recognizing and working through the grief process following rape; and (c) aiding the expression of the intrapersonal and interpersonal feelings that result from rape. 相似文献
33.
Pass K Green NS Lorey F Sherwin J Comeau AM 《Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews》2006,12(4):293-300
The term "pilot study" has been used over the years to describe the evaluation of the many elements involved in deciding whether a proposed condition should be added to a newborn screening (NBS) panel, and until recently, was unilaterally used to describe the evaluation of the assay to be used before the condition was officially adopted by a state for its newborn screening panel. Since Guthrie's introduction of screening for PKU, each time a new condition was added to the panel, the screening assay itself was validated through a population-based trial, in which the test was performed with de-identified samples to avoid association between the test result and the infant. This is considered by the laboratory as the "pilot phase" of adding a new condition. To advance the science of NBS, especially to accommodate new technologies that may provide new types of information (genetic versus physiological) for each new condition, pilot programs are essential. Involvement of the clinical community serves to improve these evaluations and provides the needed clinical validation of decisions made as a result of it. This paper describes the historical context of pilot programs in population-based NBS that utilize laboratory-based markers as indicators of concern; specifically, three applications that demonstrate different approaches to the use of pilots in adding conditions to a NBS panel are described. 相似文献
34.
35.
We tested the hypothesis that a psychosocial dental intervention formulated in terms of self-determination theory would increase
patients’ perceived competence and autonomous motivation for dental care and would decrease their plaque and gingivitis over
a seven month period, compared to standard dental treatment. We also tested a process model in which the intervention was
expected to increase perceived dental competence and autonomous motivation, that they would be positively associated with
oral health behaviors (i.e., brushing and flossing), which was expected to decrease plaque and, in turn, decrease gingivitis.
We also examined whether: changes in perceived competence and autonomous motivation would mediate the effect of the intervention
on dental-health behaviors; dental-health behaviors would mediate the links from changes in perceived competence and autonomous
motivation to change in plaque; and change in plaque would mediate the relation of dental health behaviors to change in gingivitis.
Finally, we examined the fit of the overall model with structural equation modelling. Results supported all predictions.
相似文献
Anne E. Münster HalvariEmail: |
36.
Age differences in fluid intelligence: contributions of general slowing and frontal decline 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The current study examined the contributions of general slowing and frontal decline to age differences in fluid intelligence. Participants aged 20-89 years completed Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, simple reaction time, choice reaction time, Wisconsin Card Sorting, and Tower of London tasks. Age-related declines in fluid intelligence, speed of processing, and frontal function were observed. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that the processing speed and frontal function measures accounted for significant variance in fluid intelligence performance, but there was also a residual effect of age after controlling for each variable individually as well as both variables. An additional analysis showed that the variance in fluid intelligence that was attributable to processing speed was not fully shared with the variance attributable to frontal function. These findings suggest that the age-related decline in fluid intelligence is due to general slowing and frontal decline, as well as other unidentified factors. 相似文献
37.
Cary E. Jenson Winston Turner Sue Amero Anne Johnson Gail Werrbach 《Journal of child and family studies》2002,11(3):361-371
Our study represents a rural case management agency's use of its data to aid in its transition from being funded by federal grant funds to self-sustainment. We wanted to learn if some characteristics of those children served could predict the resources needed to serve them. We analyzed data on 90 children and adolescents living in a rural community. Using multiple regression analysis we found that CAFAS total and CBCL total problem scores predicted Medicaid reimbursements but not flexible funds spending or case management hours. History of psychiatric hospitalization, however, predicted flexible funds spending and case management hours. Using one-way ANOVA and post hoc analysis we found that the staff members' assessments of each child and family's level of service needs were reflected in differences in mean CAFAS total scores, Medicaid reimbursements and case management hours, but not CBCL total problem scores or flexible funds spending. 相似文献
38.
Two experiments are reported in which the effects of presentation modality on false memory in recall and recognition are studied. False recall and recognition of critical targets are lower for non-presented items related to a study list when that study list is presented visually than when presented auditorily. This pattern of low levels of false memory for critical targets holds even when participants read the visually presented study items aloud. These results suggest that recollection of visual detail plays a role in the prevention of false memory. However, both the hit rates (true memory) and the false-alarm rates to weakly related distractors (non-critical targets) were higher for visual presentation than for auditory presentation, suggesting that more than one mechanism may underlie false recognition. 相似文献
39.
One hundred ninety-five boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were compared with 73 comparison boys (M = 9.83 years, SD = 1.30) on self-perceptions relative to a teacher-rated criterion. Emphasis was placed on ADHD subgroup comparisons according to level of aggression, academic achievement, and depression compared with control boys. Consistent with the authors' prediction, ADHD boys overestimated relative to teacher report, more than did controls, in the scholastic competence, social acceptance, and behavioral conduct domains. Examination of discrepancy scores (child rating - teacher rating) by comorbidity subgroups suggested that aggressive and low-achieving ADHD boys tended to overestimate their competence the most in the domains in which they were the most impaired. Results are discussed in terms of prior literature on "positive illusions" in ADHD children. 相似文献
40.
High- and low-trait socially anxious individuals classified the emotional expressions of photographic quality continua of interpolated ("morphed") facial images that were derived from combining 6 basic prototype emotional expressions to various degrees, with the 2 adjacent emotions arranged in an emotion hexagon. When fear was 1 of the 2 component emotions, the high-trait group displayed enhanced sensitivity for fear. In a 2nd experiment where a mood manipulation was incorporated, again, the high-trait group exhibited enhanced sensitivity for fear. The low-trait group was sensitive for happiness in the control condition. The mood-manipulated group had increased sensitivity for anger expressions, and trait anxiety did not moderate these effects. Interpretations of the results related to the classification of fearful expressions are discussed. 相似文献