全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1494篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
1505篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 36篇 |
2014年 | 23篇 |
2013年 | 177篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 63篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 47篇 |
2007年 | 47篇 |
2006年 | 53篇 |
2005年 | 44篇 |
2004年 | 50篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 61篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 29篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 21篇 |
1988年 | 25篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 24篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 26篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1505条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Many competitions, such as political campaigns, innovative endeavors, sports, or courting rituals, require investment of nonrefundable resources: One contestant wins the prize for the invested amount, whereas all others lose their investments without receiving compensation. Frequently, contests are asymmetric, because of differing resources or prize valuations. Does asymmetry discourage the weaker contestants from investing? Do stronger contestants take this into account and also invest less? Or does the desire to win abolish these differences? Two experiments explored the effects of asymmetry between the contestants—arising from their resources or prizes—on nonrefundable investments. Subjects repeatedly competed in both symmetric and asymmetric contests, enabling within‐subject comparisons. We observed an effect of asymmetry only when it concerned resources: Subjects overall invested less when their resources were unequal, whereas inequality in the prizes did not influence investments. This pattern suggests that winning itself is motivating. The changes between consecutive investments can be explained by reactions to the previous outcome (win or loss): Across all contests, investment decreased following a win and increased following a loss. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
212.
Judith M. Ottoson 《New Directions for Adult & Continuing Education》1997,1997(75):87-96
The transfer of learning to the workplace continues to be an issue of concern to trainers and educators in all venues; in this chapter the author explains the benefits of using multiple lenses to assess training effectiveness. 相似文献
213.
Judith Harel David Oppenheim Emanuel Tirosh Motti Gini 《Infant mental health journal》1999,20(2):123-137
Associations between mother–child interaction at 12 months and children's feature knowledge of self and mother at 20 months were investigated in 81 dyads. Mother–Child interaction was assessed by scales adapted from the Parent Child Early Relational Assessment (1985). Self‐ and mother knowledge were assessed by Self‐ and Mother Feature Knowledge Scales. Associations were found between mother–child interaction and later self‐ and mother feature knowledge. Maternal behavior characterized by positive affect and sensitive responsiveness and child behavior characterized by positive affect, responsiveness, and balanced attention between mother and environment were related to a more complex knowledge of self and mother. On sensorimotor items children knew more about self than about mother while on representational items children knew more about mother than about self. The findings support the view that more harmonious interactions are related to more complex self‐ and mother feature knowledge. 相似文献
214.
The aim of this study was to explore, in depth, the perceptions and experiences of Further Education (FE) amongst the young long‐term unemployed. Semi‐structured interviews were conducted with 16 long‐term unemployed youths of 18–25 years of age divided into three groups: those with no experience of FE; those having recently dropped out of FE; and those currently studying in FE. Grounded theoretical analysis highlighted the importance of both responses to and institutional aspects of unemployment. The impact of unemployment, poor previous educational experience and perceived irrelevance of FE are key barriers to learning. Those that enter FE may drop out as a result of both material and psychosocial factors. Those that stay on commonly emphasize both social support and personal agency in decision‐making alongside positive evaluations of FE. In conclusion, limitations of the findings, policy and practical recommendations for successful widening of participation in FE amongst this group are considered. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
215.
Chaos theory is presented for counselors working with clients experiencing life transitions. It is proposed as a model that considers disorder, unpredictability, and lack of control as normal parts of transition processes. Nonlinear constructs from physics are adapted for use in counseling. The model provides a method clients can use to reconstruct their own alternative life narratives, a framework in which life stories are understood, and a means to facilitate cocreation of adaptive interventions. 相似文献
216.
Rebecca L. Toporek Judith A. Lewis Hugh C. Crethar 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2009,87(3):260-268
In 2003, the American Counseling Association (ACA) adopted the ACA Advocacy Competencies (J. A. Lewis, M. S. Arnold, R. House, & R. L. Toporek, 2002) to provide guidance to counselors and acknowledge advocacy as an ethical aspect of service to clients. This article provides a foundation for this special section by sharing a historical perspective on recent pivotal advocacy movements within the profession. An overview of the development and content of the Advocacy Competencies is provided followed by a case example to assist counselors in understanding and enhancing their application. 相似文献
217.
We examined whether a salient gender identity activates gender stereotypes along the dimensions of sociability and ability (Fiske et al. 2002). A sample of US undergraduates (40 men, 38 women) instructed to think about women subsequently took longer to name the colors of words associated with sociability than ability on a modified Stroop task. Solo women in another sample of US undergraduates (45 women) showed the same response pattern. Women in a third sample of US adults (20 men, 16 women) showed a similar pattern. Meta-analysis of the three samples suggests women with a salient gender identity experience relative activation of only the positive dimension of a stereotype (e.g. “woman” equals warm). 相似文献
218.
Muris P Meesters C Mayer B Bogie N Luijten M Geebelen E Bessems J Smit C 《Behaviour research and therapy》2003,41(5):597-617
The Koala Fear Questionnaire (KFQ) is a standardized self-report scale for assessing fears and fearfulness in children aged between 4 and 12 years. The current article presents six studies which examined the reliability and validity of the KFQ. Study 1 (N=108) demonstrated that the visual fear scales of Koala bears as employed in the KFQ are highly comparable to the standard 3-point scales that are used in other childhood fear measures. Study 2 (N=163) provided support for the convergent validity of the KFQ in a sample of 8- to 14-year-old children. That is, the scale correlated substantially with alternative measures of childhood fear and anxiety. Study 3 (N=189) showed that the KFQ possesses good internal consistency and test-retest stability in a group of 8- to 11-year-old children. The results of Studies 4 (N=129) and 5 (N=176) indicated that the KFQ is suitable for children aged 4 to 6 years and demonstrated that the psychometric properties of the scale in younger children are highly similar to those obtained in older children. Study 6 (N=926) showed that the factor structure of the KFQ was theoretically meaningful: although the data clearly pointed in the direction of one factor of general fearfulness, spurs of the commonly found five-factor solution of childhood fear were found in the KFQ. Altogether, the KFQ seems to be a valuable addition to the instrumentarium of clinicians and researchers who are working with fearful and anxious children. 相似文献
219.
Lauren Humphreys Mario Cappelli Ethel Aronovitch Judith Allanson Alasdair G. W. Hunter 《Journal of applied social psychology》2008,38(2):482-512
This study explored how aspects of the couple relationship contributed to the adjustment of 95 women who underwent prenatal diagnostic (PND) testing because of advanced maternal age (AMA). Participants completed self‐report measures prior to genetic counseling (T1), during the waiting period for PND test results (T2), and after normal results were received (T3). At T2, distress was predicted by the degree to which women approached the PND decision jointly with their partners, and marital adjustment was predicted by women's satisfaction with the support received from their partners. Marital adjustment at T3 was predicted by earlier levels of partner agreement about PND‐related issues. Results suggest that the couple relationship has the potential to facilitate or hinder women's experience of PND testing. 相似文献
220.
Effects of Pre‐Trial Publicity and Jury Deliberation on Juror Bias and Source Memory Errors. By Christine Ruva, Cathy McEvoy and Judith Becker Bryant. The above article (DOI:10.1002/acp.1254) was published online in Early View on 21 June 2006. An error has subsequently been identified in this article. The mediation analyses for the critical SM errors mistakenly reported that the Sobel test z was not significant when in fact it was, Sobel test z = 2.17 (1982). Therefore, critical SM errors were found to mediate the effect of PTP on guilt ratings suggesting that these errors may be a mechanism through which PTP imparts its biasing effects on juror decision making. 相似文献