全文获取类型
收费全文 | 503篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
511篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 6篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有511条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
Jayne E. Freeman Judi A. Ellis 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(4):298-309
In young adults information designated for future enactment is more readily accessible from memory than information not intended for enactment (e.g. Goschke & Kuhl, 1993). We examined whether this advantage for to-be-enacted material is reduced in older adults and thus whether attenuated action accessibility could underlie age-associated declines in prospective remembering. Young and older adults showed an equivalent increase in accessibility (faster recognition latencies) for to-be-enacted items over items intended for verbal report. Both age groups also showed increased accessibility for actions performed at encoding compared with verbally encoded items. Moreover, these effects were non-additive, suggesting similarities in the representation of completed and to-be-completed actions. 相似文献
242.
243.
244.
Weanling male hooded rats were handled or not handled for 10 min each day, 5 days/week, for 6 weeks. At the end of this time all animals received one of the following: a septal lesion, a medial accumbens lesion, a medial hypothalamic lesion, or a sham lesion. The animals were tested for defensiveness toward the experimenter on Days 3, 7, and 14 postoperatively. All behavioral testing was done blind. Each of the three lesions increased defensiveness toward the experimenter. However, with each lesion, there was no difference in the defensiveness scores of preoperatively handled and nonhandled animals at any of the postoperative test sessions while those of animals with medial hypothalamic lesions did not. These results are consistent with observations that hyperdefensiveness occurs in human subjects following tumors in each of these brain areas in spite of their familiarity with the situation in which they are observed. They contrast with observations that mouse killing induced by septal lesions is readily prevented by preoperative exposure to a mouse. 相似文献
245.
Walsh Benjamin M. Kabat-Farr Dana Matthews Russell A. Schulte Benjamin D. 《Journal of business and psychology》2021,36(5):841-856
Journal of Business and Psychology - To prevent workplace incivility, scholars encourage organizations to use reference checks to help eliminate uncivil applicants. However, under certain... 相似文献
246.
247.
James Igoe Walsh 《Political psychology》2015,36(5):507-523
Precision weapons such as drones have become important elements of the military strategies of the United States and other countries. How does the use of precision weapons influence public support for the use of force? The public is averse to casualties, mission failure, and collateral damage. I argue that precision weapons increase the salience and importance of avoiding civilian harm. Individuals adopt their expectations about the outcomes of using these weapons and have lower tolerance for attacks that result in civilian deaths. This proposition is consistent with the results of two survey experiments. In the first, the possibility of civilian casualties leads to larger declines in support for the use of force than do military casualties or mission failure. In the second, respondents primed with information about an attack with precision weapons exhibited less tolerance for civilian harm than those primed with other weapons systems, despite the fact that the outcomes described to all respondents were identical. 相似文献
248.
249.
250.
Emily Walsh Lia Rosenstein Kristy Dalrymple Iwona Chelminski Mark Zimmerman 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2017,24(3-4):341-354
For bariatric surgery candidates, history of child abuse and PTSD may be under-recognized or under-reported at pre-surgical evaluation. On a range of clinically relevant factors, we studied 3045 candidates for bariatric surgery: (1) those with a history of childhood abuse compared to those without such history; and (2) among candidates with a history of abuse, those with a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD compared to those without that diagnosis. We compared them on current and lifetime eating disorders, physical health problems, health behaviors, physical functioning, psychosocial functioning, psychiatric disorders, emotional wellness, body satisfaction, and self-esteem. We hypothesized that patients with a history of childhood abuse, and within that group, those with a lifetime PTSD diagnosis, would display greater overall impairment. Patients were interviewed with semi-structured interviews and completed self-report questionnaires. Results showed that (1) patients with a history of childhood abuse exhibited significantly greater impairment than those without abuse; and (2) among candidates with a history of abuse, those with a lifetime history of PTSD displayed significantly greater impairment than those without a PTSD diagnosis. The findings suggest that a history of both childhood abuse and lifetime PTSD should be thoroughly assessed for at pre-surgical evaluation, and that greater attention be paid to the experience of PTSD symptoms in abuse survivors presenting for bariatric surgery. 相似文献