首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1596篇
  免费   94篇
  国内免费   1篇
  1691篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   26篇
  2016年   40篇
  2015年   35篇
  2014年   51篇
  2013年   271篇
  2012年   36篇
  2011年   26篇
  2010年   28篇
  2009年   48篇
  2008年   30篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   24篇
  2004年   29篇
  2003年   22篇
  2002年   28篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   24篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   25篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   29篇
  1992年   21篇
  1991年   22篇
  1990年   21篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   26篇
  1987年   20篇
  1986年   26篇
  1985年   21篇
  1984年   26篇
  1983年   25篇
  1982年   25篇
  1981年   30篇
  1980年   17篇
  1979年   27篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   18篇
  1975年   17篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   15篇
  1972年   16篇
  1971年   16篇
  1969年   18篇
  1968年   15篇
  1967年   18篇
  1966年   27篇
  1964年   17篇
  1962年   16篇
排序方式: 共有1691条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
912.
913.
914.
915.
916.
917.
To promote understanding of young people behavioural mechanisms related to health, we evaluated religious attribution, meaning in life, and emotion regulation strategies as predictors of life satisfaction in the sample of 791 religious and non-religious American college students. Participants completed Religious Attribution Scale, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Satisfaction with Life Scale, and the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. A multiple linear regression revealed that the strongest predictor of life satisfaction for both groups was the presence of meaning in life, followed by the use of cognitive reappraisal. We found that participants with high levels of presence of meaning in life, regardless of their religious identity, were able to benefit from the use of cognitive reappraisal. Presence of meaning in life and emotion regulation strategy reside outside of the religious confines, supporting the notion that religiosity is not a singular meaning making system. By identifying constructs related to life satisfaction, we can acquire better understanding of what mechanisms contribute to positive coping strategies that religious and non-religious young adults use to achieve positive life outcomes.  相似文献   
918.
The observation that older adults show enhanced cognition for emotionally positive information has been labeled the positivity effect (Reed, Chan, & Mikels, 2014). According to the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST, Carstensen, 1991), a prominent lifespan development theory, cognition is strongly influenced by motivational goals, and these goals are impacted by subjective time perspective. Although the positivity effect is most commonly observed in older adults, as age usually co-varies with time perspective, the SST posits that time perspective, not age, is the key explanatory factor of positivity. We examined the effects of these predictors on positivity in an episodic memory task in younger and older adults and found that age, not time perspective, was a key predictor of memory positivity. Our results add to the growing literature that challenge the notion that time perspective is the driving force behind age-related differences in emotional processing and functioning.  相似文献   
919.
Does hindsight knowledge make research seem more ethical and predictable? In line with the notion of hindsight bias, students in 3 experiments knowing the outcome of an animal experiment judged the results as more foreseeable and ethical relative to students who did not know the outcome. Via self to other comparisons, students evaluate themselves more favorably compared to a peer but exhibited hindsight bias in doing so. Uniquely, the findings reveal the possibility that students deem themselves to be more skeptical and objective than their peers. Implications for teaching animal ethics and for Institutional Review Boards are discussed.  相似文献   
920.
We compared the effects of video prompting (VP) with and without voice‐over narration on the play skills of two young children with autism spectrum disorder using an adapted alternating treatment design. The results showed increases in play skills when using VP with and without voice‐over narration for both participants. These results suggest that the difference in effectiveness between VP with and without voice‐over narration may be negligible for some children with autism. Implications for the use of VP without additional teaching strategies and the need for additional component analyses are discussed. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号