全文获取类型
收费全文 | 692篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
700篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 85篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 29篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 32篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有700条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
551.
Joyce Kloc McClure 《The Journal of religious ethics》2003,31(1):23-44
The author takes up a provocative question poised by Charles Taylor about the relationship between our commitments to a good such as neighbor love and the possibilities of achieving and sustaining social justice. Taylor's concern is not only that we make such a commitment but that we make it in such a way that we avoid its ability to lead us towards injustice rather than justice. After articulating conceptions of love, justice, and injustice, the author turns to Charles Dickens's treatment of love and injustice in Bleak House , to explore more fully how love can lead to injustice, and also its potential role in promoting justice. Dickens's view, profoundly shaped by his own sense of Christian virtues, helps us see the inner workings of love, justice, and injustice, so that we can appreciate their interconnectedness anew and understand better the urgency of Taylor's question for our time. 相似文献
552.
553.
554.
William Blake Erickson James Michael Lampinen Juliana K. Leding 《Applied cognitive psychology》2014,28(3):349-359
When an eyewitness suffers an impairment of memory for a perpetrator because the criminal used a weapon during the crime, this impairment is called the weapon focus effect. The literature is split on how this arises: Some implicate the narrowing of attentional cues to the weapon because the arousal of the victim increases, whereas others claim that the weapon is a novel object in most everyday contexts, and novel objects demand more attention than contextually appropriate ones. The current study employed a simulated crime paradigm featuring a normal, novel, or threatening object. Timing of the object's presentation was manipulated such that it was visible before, after, or during the time when the culprit's face was visible. Target‐present and target‐absent line‐ups as well as retrospective questions were administered. Both the novel object and the weapon resulted in increased mistaken identifications in target‐absent line‐ups. Structural equation modeling suggested that object novelty mediated this effect. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
555.
556.
557.
558.
Two fundamental issues in emotion theory and research concern: (a) the role of emotion in promoting response coherence across different emotion systems; and (b) the role of awareness of bodily sensations in the experience of emotion. The present study poses a question bridging the two domains; namely, whether training in Vipassana meditation or dance, both of which promote attention to certain kinds of bodily sensations, is associated with greater coherence between the subjective and physiological aspects of emotion. We used lag correlations to examine second-by-second coherence between subjective emotional experience and heart period within individuals across four different films. Participants were either: (a) experienced Vipassana meditators (attention to visceral sensations), (b) experienced dancers (attention to somatic sensations), and (c) controls with no meditation or dance experience. Results indicated a linear relationship in coherence, with meditators having highest levels, dancers having intermediary levels, and controls having lowest levels. We conclude that the coherence between subjective and cardiac aspects of emotion is greater in those who have specialized training that promotes greater body awareness. 相似文献
559.
The relationship between early life events and the socialized and undersocialized dimensions of conduct disorder was investigated. Subjects were 40 incarcerated male delinquents and their mothers. The mothers were administered the Children's Life Events Questionnaire and the Developmental Questionnaire. Youths were administered the Adolescent Parent Relations Scale. The results indicated that the undersocialized delinquents had a greater number of stressful life events during their first 4 years of life than did the socialized delinquents. Discriminant function analysis indicated that stressful life events during the 2nd and 4th years were the most important predictors of membership in the undersocialized group. The discriminant function generated was able to classify 82.5% of the sample correctly. The remaining variables did not significantly discriminate the groups. 相似文献
560.
Tess A. Shirefley Claudia L. Castañeda Joyce Rodriguez-Gutiérrez Jennifer Jipson 《Journal of cognition and development》2020,21(4):551-572
ABSTRACT Family conversations about science-related topics, including those involving storybook reading, may set the stage for children’s interest in science. We investigated how parents from two cultural backgrounds engaged in science talk while reading a science-related storybook with their preschool-aged daughters and sons. Consistent with our commitment to avoid deficit thinking, our questions focus on variability within a group of European-American parents and a group of Latine parents, rather than comparing groups. Our sample included 38 European-American families (20 girls), and 27 Latine families (12 girls) from three coastal regions of California, varying in educational background. Our results indicate that parents from both groups read most of the text, had conversations beyond the text, and specifically engaged in science talk when elaborating beyond the book with their children. European-American parents with 12–16 years of schooling engaged in science talk more with boys than with girls, whereas the science talk of European-American parents with more than 16 years of schooling did not differ by children’s gender. Latine parents did not differ significantly in their science talk by years of parents’ schooling or by children’s gender. Results are discussed as they relate to current conversations about early socialization of science interest and engagement for boys and girls in diverse families. 相似文献