全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1977篇 |
免费 | 167篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
2147篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 68篇 |
2019年 | 72篇 |
2018年 | 104篇 |
2017年 | 102篇 |
2016年 | 131篇 |
2015年 | 40篇 |
2014年 | 72篇 |
2013年 | 215篇 |
2012年 | 109篇 |
2011年 | 127篇 |
2010年 | 67篇 |
2009年 | 67篇 |
2008年 | 92篇 |
2007年 | 82篇 |
2006年 | 75篇 |
2005年 | 47篇 |
2004年 | 48篇 |
2003年 | 52篇 |
2002年 | 36篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 27篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1969年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2147条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Neurofibromatosis, type I (NF-I) is associated with verbal and nonverbal neuropsychological deficits and neuroanatomical anomalies. Few relationships between CNS abnormalities and cognitive function in this population, however, have been found. Reading disabilities and developmental language impairments in the general population have been associated with particular morphologic features in inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and Heschl's gyrus (HG). We compared the morphology of these regions in children with NF-I and controls. Verbal skills in NF-I were related to IFG morphology, such that individuals with NF-I who showed "typical" gyral patterns in the right hemisphere performed worse across language measures than those showing an extra "atypical" gyrus. A doubling of HG in the left and right hemispheres was also significantly associated with performance on several neuropsychological measures. This is the first study to link regional gyral morphology with language function in NF-I. A possible molecular basis for the observed relationships is discussed. 相似文献
92.
Now you feel it, now you don't: frontal brain electrical asymmetry and individual differences in emotion regulation 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Jackson DC Mueller CJ Dolski I Dalton KM Nitschke JB Urry HL Rosenkranz MA Ryff CD Singer BH Davidson RJ 《Psychological science》2003,14(6):612-617
Recent theoretical accounts of emotion regulation assign an important role in this process to the prefrontal cortex, yet there is little relevant data available to support this hypothesis. The current study assessed the relation between individual differences in asymmetric prefrontal activation and an objective measure of uninstructed emotion regulation. Forty-seven participants 57 to 60 years old viewed emotionally arousing and neutral visual stimuli while eyeblink startle data were collected. Startle probes were also presented after picture presentation to capture the persistence or attenuation of affect following the offset of an emotional stimulus. Subjects with greater relative left-sided anterior activation in scalp-recorded brain electrical signals displayed attenuated startle magnitude after the offset of negative stimuli. This relation between resting frontal activation and recovery following an aversive event supports the idea of a frontally mediated mechanism involved in one form of automatic emotion regulation. 相似文献
93.
Jackson JP 《History of psychology》2003,6(2):195-202
In a recent article, Howard H. Kendler (2002) criticized Kenneth B. Clark and the other social scientists who worked in Brown v. Board of Education for polluting their science with value judgments. This article argues that Kendler's critique is misguided because it conflates a policy judgment with a value judgment. In addition, Kendler inconsistently applied his own standard of objectivity when he examined in the social science used in Brown. 相似文献
94.
Differences in psychosocial experiences of employed, unemployed, and student samples of young adults
Jackson T 《The Journal of psychology》1999,133(1):49-60
The author conducted this cross-sectional study to assess the extent to which employed, unemployed, and student samples of young adults could be differentiated on the basis of psychosocial experiences. Forty-four employed, 41 unemployed, and 42 student volunteers completed measures of emotional distress, time structure, financial strain, skill use, task variety, social support, and demographic information. After the effects of educational and income differences were statistically controlled, results indicated that unemployed participants reported poorer functioning than employed and student respondents on most measures. In a discriminant function analysis, 66% of the sample was correctly identified from scores on psychosocial measures, with higher classification rates for employed and unemployed participants. The findings provide additional support for contextual models of mental health and suggest that full-time education partially buffers the negative psychological consequences of unemployment when opportunities for employment are absent. 相似文献
95.
Jackson LC 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》1999,5(1):27-36
Teaching diversity courses in mental health programs presents a unique set of issues for the faculty. These courses generate various forms of emotional reactions in students that could take the form of anger, silence, avoidance, and passivity. The purpose of this article is to specifically focus on the experience of students of color who find themselves in these courses and the various ways they respond. This learning process is often impeded by resistance because of the personal experiences of all students, but students of color experience a unique set of resistances in each stage of this process, either because of their own experience or because of a lack of experience with racism, racial and biracial identity development, cultural and bicultural identity, or acculturation issues. Resistance in the classroom interferes with the reciprocal communication between instructors and students and interferes with learning and the development of trust between instructors and students of color. Suggestions are made to help faculty to understand these resistances and to develop appropriate responses for working through the process. 相似文献
96.
Examined the influence of the quality of the teacher-student relationship on children's subsequent levels of aggression in a sample of 61 second and third-grade children nominated and rated by their teachers as aggressive. The stability of teachers' and children's reports of relationships quality across academic years was in the low to moderate range. Teachers and children showed little agreement in their reports of relationship quality. Teachers' and children's reports of relationship quality in year 1 (Y1) predicted teacher-rated aggression the following year, controlling for Y1 ratings of aggression. Teachers' reports of relationship quality across Y1 and Y2 predicted peer-rated aggression, but not teacher-rated aggression, in Y3, controlling for Y1 aggression. Consistent with a buffering role for teacher-student relationships, a positive teacher-student relationship was of greatest benefit to children whose mothers reported rejecting parenting histories. We discuss implications for school-based prevention programs. 相似文献
97.
Joshua M. Langberg Stephen P. Becker Melissa R. Dvorsky 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2014,42(1):91-103
The purpose of the study was to evaluate the relation between Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) and academic functioning in a sample of 52 adolescents (40 males, 12 females) with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD; M age?=?13.75). This study builds on prior work by utilizing an empirically-based and psychometrically validated measure of SCT, collecting ratings of SCT from both parents and teachers, and examining associations with multiple domains of academic functioning from both the parent and teacher perspective as well as grade point average (GPA). Both SCT and DSM-IV symptoms of inattention were significantly correlated with domains of academic functioning. Multiple regression analyses demonstrated that the parent-rated SCT Slow subscale predicted overall academic functioning, organizational skills impairment, and homework problems above and beyond ADHD symptoms and child and demographic characteristics known to be associated with academics, including intelligence, academic achievement, and family income. The teacher-rated SCT Low Initiation/Persistence subscale also predicted homework problems and was the only SCT variable to predict school grades above and beyond ADHD symptoms and relevant covariates. Both the SCT Slow and Low Initiation/Persistence subscales include items related to youth seeming apathetic, unmotivated, and lacking initiative, behaviors that are strongly related to ADHD symptoms of inattention but not currently captured by the DSM-IV. Implications of these findings towards supporting the external validity of the SCT construct are discussed along with potential implications for intervention. 相似文献
98.
Daniel N. Osherson Joshua Stern Ormond Wilkie Michael Stob Edward E. Smith 《Cognitive Science》1991,15(2):251-269
A probability may be called “default” if it is neither derived from preestablished probabilities nor based on considerations of frequency or symmetry. Default probabilities presumably arise through reasoning based on causality and similarity. This article advances a model of default probability based on a featural approach to similarity. The accuracy of the model is assessed by comparing its predictions to the probabilities provided by undergraduates asked to reason about mammals. 相似文献
99.
Odegard TN Koen JD Gama JM 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2008,34(5):1296-1304
A surge of research has been conducted to examine memory editing mechanisms that help distinguish accurate from inaccurate memories. In the present experiment, the authors examined the ability of participants to use novelty detection, recollection rejection, and plausibility judgments to reject lures presented on a recognition memory test. Participants studied a list of word pairs that were arranged in a category relationship (both words from the same category) or an unrelated relationship (both words from different categories) under full or divided attention. At test, participants were given a yes/no recognition test in which they were to respond after seeing the test items for 400 ms or 2,800 ms. Some of the test items were rearranged word pairs that were consistent with the study relationship, whereas others were inconsistent with the study relationship. The results demonstrate that the participants required full attention at study to use novelty detection, recollection rejection, and plausibility judgments to reject lures. Moreover, the results indicate that a long response deadline at test was needed for participants to use both recollection rejection and plausibility judgments to reject lures. 相似文献
100.
Functional impulsivity and reinforcement sensitivity theory 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1