首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   180005篇
  免费   8036篇
  国内免费   157篇
  2020年   2795篇
  2019年   3446篇
  2018年   3542篇
  2017年   4001篇
  2016年   4626篇
  2015年   3931篇
  2014年   4817篇
  2013年   23543篇
  2012年   4683篇
  2011年   3748篇
  2010年   3892篇
  2009年   4791篇
  2008年   3988篇
  2007年   3517篇
  2006年   4099篇
  2005年   4106篇
  2004年   3566篇
  2003年   3197篇
  2002年   3000篇
  2001年   3547篇
  2000年   3408篇
  1999年   3309篇
  1998年   2857篇
  1997年   2682篇
  1996年   2610篇
  1995年   2442篇
  1994年   2397篇
  1993年   2362篇
  1992年   2821篇
  1991年   2644篇
  1990年   2526篇
  1989年   2388篇
  1988年   2361篇
  1987年   2388篇
  1986年   2384篇
  1985年   2637篇
  1984年   2651篇
  1983年   2424篇
  1982年   2408篇
  1981年   2393篇
  1980年   2210篇
  1979年   2374篇
  1978年   2245篇
  1977年   2214篇
  1976年   2028篇
  1975年   2183篇
  1974年   2262篇
  1973年   2167篇
  1972年   1744篇
  1971年   1666篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
181.
182.
183.
184.
To understand how individuals adapt to and anticipate each other in joint tasks, we employ a bidirectional delay–coupled dynamical system that allows for mutual adaptation and anticipation. In delay–coupled systems, anticipation is achieved when one system compares its own time‐delayed behavior, which implicitly includes past information about the other system’s behavior, with the other system’s instantaneous behavior. Applied to joint music performance, the model allows each system to adapt its behavior to the dynamics of the other. Model predictions of asynchrony between two simultaneously produced musical voices were compared with duet pianists’ behavior; each partner performed one voice while auditory feedback perturbations occurred at unpredictable times during live performance. As the model predicted, when auditory feedback from one musical voice was removed, the asynchrony changed: The pianist’s voice that was removed anticipated (preceded) the actions of their partner. When the auditory feedback returned and both musicians could hear each other, they rapidly returned to baseline levels of asynchrony. To understand how the pianists anticipated each other, their performances were fitted by the model to examine change in model parameters (coupling strength, time‐delay). When auditory feedback for one or both voices was removed, the fits showed the expected decrease in coupling strength and time‐delay between the systems. When feedback about the voice(s) returned, the coupling strength and time‐delay returned to baseline. These findings support the idea that when people perform actions together, they do so as a coupled bidirectional anticipatory system.  相似文献   
185.
186.
187.
188.
189.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
190.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号