全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4741篇 |
免费 | 105篇 |
专业分类
4846篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 59篇 |
2019年 | 85篇 |
2018年 | 113篇 |
2017年 | 137篇 |
2016年 | 137篇 |
2015年 | 85篇 |
2014年 | 113篇 |
2013年 | 553篇 |
2012年 | 160篇 |
2011年 | 159篇 |
2010年 | 109篇 |
2009年 | 116篇 |
2008年 | 185篇 |
2007年 | 161篇 |
2006年 | 128篇 |
2005年 | 135篇 |
2004年 | 125篇 |
2003年 | 120篇 |
2002年 | 98篇 |
2001年 | 62篇 |
2000年 | 72篇 |
1999年 | 63篇 |
1998年 | 73篇 |
1997年 | 52篇 |
1996年 | 54篇 |
1995年 | 48篇 |
1994年 | 57篇 |
1992年 | 63篇 |
1990年 | 53篇 |
1989年 | 56篇 |
1988年 | 51篇 |
1987年 | 36篇 |
1986年 | 55篇 |
1985年 | 50篇 |
1984年 | 67篇 |
1983年 | 53篇 |
1982年 | 51篇 |
1981年 | 61篇 |
1980年 | 59篇 |
1979年 | 48篇 |
1978年 | 52篇 |
1977年 | 63篇 |
1976年 | 55篇 |
1975年 | 67篇 |
1974年 | 55篇 |
1973年 | 63篇 |
1972年 | 38篇 |
1971年 | 40篇 |
1970年 | 41篇 |
1968年 | 55篇 |
排序方式: 共有4846条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Stemberger JP 《Brain and language》2004,90(1-3):413-422
Models of speech production differ on whether phonological neighbourhoods should affect processing, and on whether effects should be facilitatory or inhibitory. Inhibitory effects of large neighbourhoods have been argued to underlie apparent anti-frequency effects, whereby high-frequency default features are more prone to mispronunciation errors than low-frequency nondefault features. Data from the original SLIPs experiments that found apparent anti-frequency effects are analysed for neighbourhood effects. Effects are facilitatory: errors are significantly less likely for words with large numbers of neighbours that share the characteristic that is being primed for error ("friends"). Words in the neighbourhood that do not share the target characteristic ("enemies") have little effect on error rates. Neighbourhood effects do not underlie the apparent anti-frequency effects. Implications for models of speech production are discussed. 相似文献
162.
Clare J Lewandowsky S 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2004,30(4):739-755
This article investigated the role of the recognition criterion in the verbal overshadowing effect (VOE). In 3 experiments, people witnessed an event, verbally described a perpetrator, and then attempted identification. The authors found in Experiment 1, which included a "not present" response option and both perpetrator-present (PP) and perpetrator-absent (PA) lineups, an increased reluctance to identify a person from both lineup types after verbalization. Experiment 2 incorporated a forced-choice procedure, and the authors found no effect of verbalization on identification performance. Experiment 3 replicated the essential aspects of these results. Consequently, the VOE may reflect a change in recognition criterion rather than a changed processing style or alteration of the underlying memory trace. This conclusion was confirmed by computational modeling of the data. 相似文献
163.
Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Reductions in latent inhibition (LI), the capacity to screen from conscious awareness stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant, have been generally associated with the tendency towards psychosis. However, "failure" to screen out previously irrelevant stimuli might also hypothetically contribute to original thinking, particularly in combination with high IQ. Meta-analysis of two studies, conducted on youthful high-IQ samples. demonstrated that high lifetime creative achievers had significantly lower LI scores than low creative achievers (r(effect size) = .31, p = .0003, one-tailed). Eminent creative achievers (participants under 21 years who reported unusually high scores in a single domain of creative achievement) were 7 times more likely to have low rather than high LI scores, chi2 (1, N = 25) = 10.69, phi = .47. p = .003. 相似文献
164.
Character strengths before and after September 11 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Did Americans change following the September 11 terrorist attacks? We provide a tentative answer with respect to the positive traits included in the Values in Action Classification of Strengths and measured with a self-report questionnaire available on-line and completed by 4,817 respondents. When scores for individuals completing the survey in the 2 months immediately after September 11 were compared with scores for those individuals who completed the survey before September 11, seven character strengths showed increases: gratitude, hope, kindness, leadership, love, spirituality, and teamwork. Ten months after September 11, these character strengths were still elevated, although to a somewhat lesser degree than immediately following the attacks. 相似文献
165.
Nineteen psychologists and 19 graduate students scored two Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition patient protocols. Mean IQs and indexes were similar across groups, but the ranges for Verbal IQ (VIQ), Performance IQ (PIQ), and Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) on one protocol were 25, 22, and 11 points, respectively. For both protocols taken together, percentages of agreement with the "actual" IQs for psychologists were only 26.3 for VIQ, 36.8 for PIQ, and 42.1 for FSIQ. For students, percentages were 15.8 for VIQ, 23.7 for PIQ, and 31.6 for FSIQ. The percentages of FSIQs that fell within +/- 1 standard error of measurement of the actual IQs were 89.5 for psychologists and 76.3 for students. Scoring error also had a negative impact on index scores. Both groups were confident about their scoring accuracy. 相似文献
166.
There is growing dissatisfaction with a dichotomized "anger-in" versus "anger-out" view of anger coping. Three studies using student and community adultsamples revealed a broader understanding of the nature of anger coping styles and led to the development of the new Behavioral Anger Response Questionnaire (BARQ). The BARQ is empirically derived and factorially validated and has good psychometrics. Results suggest that dichotomizing anger responses as "in" versus "out" is too coarse and that a 6-factor model may be more appropriate. The 6 factors identified here are Direct Anger-Out, Assertion, Support-Seeking, Diffusion, Avoidance, and Rumination. Women reported use of a wider range of anger coping styles, especially more social support-seeking and more use of anger diffusion strategies than men. 相似文献
167.
Christopher?A.?KurbyEmail author Katja?Wiemer-Hastings Nagasai?Ganduri Joseph?P.?Magliano Keith?K.?Millis Danielle?S.?McNamara 《Behavior research methods》2003,35(2):244-250
The effectiveness of a domain-specific latent semantic analysis (LSA) in assessing reading strategies was examined. Students were given self-explanation reading training (SERT) and asked to think aloud after each sentence in a science text. Novice and expert human raters and two LSA spaces (general reading, science) rated the similarity of each think-aloud protocol to benchmarks representing three different reading strategies (minimal, local, and global). The science LSA space correlated highly with human judgments, and more highly than did the general reading space. Also, cosines from the science LSA spaces can distinguish between different levels of semantic similarity, but may have trouble in distinguishing local processing protocols. Thus, a domain-specific LSA space is advantageous regardless of the size of the space. The results are discussedin the context of applying the science LSA to a computer-based version of SERT that gives online feedback based on LSA cosines. 相似文献
168.
169.
Gregory R. Peterson 《Zygon》1999,34(1):139-149
Beginning with the End represents an excellent collection of articles devoted to the thought of Wolfhart Pannenberg. This volume includes many of the most important thinkers in the science-religion dialogue and shows as well the importance and impact of Pannenberg's theology. This response addresses themes that surface in several of the articles: What is religion? What is science? What is theology? What is God? On some of these themes there is agreement, on others sharp disagreement. The conclusion also considers what this volume suggests about the future of Pannenberg's theology. 相似文献
170.