首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   182696篇
  免费   8089篇
  国内免费   163篇
  2020年   2779篇
  2019年   3452篇
  2018年   3719篇
  2017年   4145篇
  2016年   4738篇
  2015年   3925篇
  2014年   4803篇
  2013年   23498篇
  2012年   5014篇
  2011年   4259篇
  2010年   4055篇
  2009年   4879篇
  2008年   4433篇
  2007年   4039篇
  2006年   4432篇
  2005年   4371篇
  2004年   3868篇
  2003年   3461篇
  2002年   3295篇
  2001年   3800篇
  2000年   3604篇
  1999年   3450篇
  1998年   2889篇
  1997年   2700篇
  1996年   2601篇
  1995年   2454篇
  1994年   2408篇
  1993年   2356篇
  1992年   2817篇
  1991年   2660篇
  1990年   2519篇
  1989年   2405篇
  1988年   2369篇
  1987年   2361篇
  1986年   2364篇
  1985年   2587篇
  1984年   2601篇
  1983年   2374篇
  1982年   2417篇
  1981年   2364篇
  1980年   2225篇
  1979年   2311篇
  1978年   2231篇
  1977年   2187篇
  1976年   1996篇
  1975年   2113篇
  1974年   2154篇
  1973年   2011篇
  1972年   1625篇
  1971年   1550篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 11 毫秒
971.
972.
973.
The past fifteen years have seen the development of a considerable research literature on the social psychology of procedural justice (see Lind & Tyler, 1988, for a review). Procedural justice research reveals some serious shortcomings in the exchange theories that have traditionally dominated Western analyses of the social psychology of groups, and in so doing, the procedural justice literature has important ramifications for cross-cultural psychology. Results from a number of studies conducted in the United States and Western Europe show that individualistic, self-interest based models of human behaviour are insufficient to explain procedural justice phenomena. Instead, procedural justice effects frequently reveal strong group-oriented concerns and motivations even in cultural contexts generally thought to be characterized by individualistic orientations. The research literature also shows that if a group's procedures are judged to be fair, people are more likely to show group-oriented behaviour and to hold more favourable attitudes toward the group and its leaders. These findings have led to the development of a theory of justice judgments—the Lind and Tyler group-value theory—which is based on group norms and relations rather than on social exchange theory. We describe a general model of social behaviour that integrates group- and individually-oriented behaviour, and we discuss the implications of the model for social and cross-cultural psychology.  相似文献   
974.
This study examined Protestant Work Ethic (PWE) and Just World Beliefs (JWB) in Great Britain and India. Secondary school children, university students and adults completed seven PWE questionnaires and the JWB questionnaire. A three-way (sex × occupational status × nationality) ANOVA showed highly significant main effects, particularly for nationality, but few significant interactions. Generally females more than males, Indians more than Britons, and schoolchildren more than adults endorse the PWE and JWB. Possible experimental artifacts and moderator variables are considered along with the possibility that genuine cultural differences were found. These results are discussed in terms of the relevant literature on cross-cultural studies of the PWE and BJW.  相似文献   
975.
976.
Twenty treated and 18 untreated spider phobics were exposed to a series of 72 slides. Three different categories of slides were used: phobia-relevant slides (spiders), alternative fear-relevant slides (weapons), and neutral slides (flowers). Slides were randomly paired with either a shock, a tone, or nothing at all. Despite the absence of a systematic correlation between slides and outcomes, untreated phobics strongly overestimated the covariation between spider slides and shock. Treated phobics did not show a covariation bias, suggesting that such bias can be modulated by behavioral treatment. In addition, untreated subjects were more confident about their contingency estimates than were treated subjects. The present results fit with earlier studies.  相似文献   
977.
Thinking through uncertainty: nonconsequential reasoning and choice.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
When thinking under uncertainty, people often do not consider appropriately each of the relevant branches of a decision tree, as required by consequentialism. As a result they sometimes violate Savage's sure-thing principle. In the Prisoner's Dilemma game, for example, many subjects compete when they know that the opponent has competed and when they know that the opponent has cooperated, but cooperate when they do not know the opponent's response. Newcomb's Problem and Wason's selection task are also interpreted as manifestations of nonconsequential decision making and reasoning. The causes and implications of such behavior, and the notion of quasi-magical thinking, are discussed.  相似文献   
978.
979.
980.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号